Literature
首页医源资料库在线期刊美国临床营养学杂志2000年71卷第1期

n-3 Polyunsaturated fatty acid requirements of term infants

来源:《美国临床营养学杂志》
摘要:ABSTRACTThebenchmarksforhumannutrientrequirementsaretherecommendeddietaryintakes(RDIs)。Thisisbecominganincreasingchallengewiththeintroductionofmanynutraceuticalsandfunctionalfoods,aprimeexamplebeingthedebatesurroundingtheintroductionoflong-chainpolyunsaturat......

点击显示 收起

Robert A Gibson and Maria Makrides

1 From the Child Nutrition Research Centre, Child Health Research Institute, North Adelaide, Australia, and the Department of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, University of Adelaide, Women's and Children's Hospital, North Adelaide, Australia.

2 Supported by the National Health (NH) and Medical Research Council (MRC) Public Health Research and Development Committee. RAG was partly supported by the MS McLeod Trust. MM was partly supported by an NH and MRC Applied Health Sciences Fellowship.

3 Address reprint requests to RA Gibson, Child Nutrition Research Centre and Department of Paediatrics and Child Health, Flinders Medical Centre, Bedford Park, SA 5042, Australia. E-mail: rgibson{at}flinders.edu.au.


ABSTRACT  
The benchmarks for human nutrient requirements are the recommended dietary intakes (RDIs). However, the RDIs are set to prevent a clinical deficiency state in an otherwise healthy population and there are few nutrient recommendations set with the goal of achieving an optimal or maximal state of nutrition and health. This is becoming an increasing challenge with the introduction of many nutraceuticals and functional foods, a prime example being the debate surrounding the introduction of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids (LCPUFAs) into infant formulas. Most expert nutrition committees have used the fatty acid composition of breast milk as a basis for recommendations for infant formulas, with little information on the minimum absolute requirement for essential PUFAs. It has been difficult to determine a minimum requirement for fatty acids because 1) LCPUFAs can be synthesized from precursor fatty acids, 2) plasma n-3 LCPUFA concentrations representing deficiency and sufficiency are not clearly defined, and 3) there are no recognized clinical tests for n-3 LCPUFA deficiency and sufficiency. Therefore, there is a clear need to associate a measure of LCPUFA status with a specific functional outcome before any recommendations can be made for achieving optimal or maximal LCPUFA status.

Key Words: n-3 Polyunsaturated fatty acids • long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids • infant formula • neural development • neurologic development • term infants • docosahexaenoic acid • DHA • breast milk • human milk


INTRODUCTION  
For some vitamins and minerals, there are well-defined dietary requirements. For example, there are separate recommended dietary intakes (RDIs) of iron for infants, adult males, females, and pregnant women (1). These RDIs have been determined from measures of intake that correlate with markers of iron status, such as blood hemoglobin and plasma ferritin concentrations. Individuals who consume iron-deficient diets are at risk of anemia and have reduced hemoglobin concentrations. In addition, excessive dietary iron consumption may be toxic. Between these 2 extremes is the range of adequate dietary iron intakes. At the low end of the adequate range, individuals may be classified as nonanemic iron-deficient (marginal status) with reduced plasma ferritin concentrations; such individuals are considered at risk of clinical deficiency. Although much research is still focused on defining the subtleties of nutrient requirements in specific circumstances, there is general agreement, which is reflected in a similarity of dietary requirements among various countries.

Nutritional requirements for polyunsaturated fatty acids (PUFAs), on the other hand, are not clearly defined for either adults or infants and few known dietary intake values have been equated with blood concentrations that in turn can be related to function. This is further complicated by the fact that, although we are dependent on our diets for the 2 known essential fatty acids, linoleic acid (LA, 18:2n-6) and -linolenic acid (ALA, 18:3n-3), they can also be converted to 20- and 22-carbon metabolites [long-chain PUFAs (LCPUFAs)], which have profound biological activity. The need for dietary essential fatty acids may be obviated if LCPUFAs are included in the diet.

The nutritional requirement for LA is the best known. An absence of LA from the diet results in growth retardation and dermatologic abnormalities. Although reduced dietary intake of LA can be correlated with reduced plasma concentrations of LA and elevated concentrations of mead acid (eicosatrienoic acid, 20:3n-9), no studies have associated plasma concentrations of LA or mead acid with a biological function or clinical state. Furthermore, mead acid is a product of the n-9 pathway and its concentrations can also be influenced by high oleic acid (18:1n-9) intakes (2). Dietary LA intakes of 0.6% energy as LA are thought to be adequate for avoiding essential fatty acid deficiency as indicated by the triene to tetraene ratio [ratio of mead acid to arachidonic acid (AA)], but currently most regulatory authorities recommend the provision of 7.06 µg/J (300 mg/100 kcal) or 2.7% energy as LA to infants (3).

The amount of dietary LA that is toxic is not known and infant formulas with as much as 70% total fat as LA have been marketed in the past with no reported adverse clinical effects (4). High-LA formulas are currently not recommended because of concerns about increased risk of adverse effects from LA peroxidation products and because of the known inhibition by LA of n-3 LCPUFA incorporation into cells. Interestingly, the optimum dietary intake of the long-chain derivative of LA, AA (20:4n-6), is not known, although estimates have been made (5).

In contrast to LA deficiency, ALA deficiency in humans has not been extensively documented and most clues have come from animal studies (6). For example, ALA deficiency produced alopecia and scaly dermatitis in capuchin monkeys (7) and loss of visual acuity in young rhesus monkeys (8). In humans, ALA deficiency has been described in only one 6-y-old girl who presented with neurologic and visual symptoms (9) and 5 adult patients who presented with cutaneous symptoms after either prolonged intravenous nutrition (10) or long-term gastric tube feeding (11). However, in all the human cases the symptoms attributed to ALA deficiency may have been partly accounted for by other nutritional deficiencies, such as low concentrations of vitamin E or total essential fatty acid deficiency. In rhesus monkey trials of ALA deficiency in which high concentrations of dietary LA were maintained, no cutaneous symptoms or growth failure were reported (8). Researchers have estimated that the daily ALA requirement to avoid deficiency symptoms is 0.3–0.5% of energy (9–11).

The biological benefit of ALA may be dependent on its conversion to LCPUFAs. The neurologic and visual deficits found in monkeys deficient in n-3 fatty acids have been ascribed to deficiency of docosahexaenoic acid (DHA, 22:6n-3), the longer-chain metabolite of ALA, because neural deficits in the monkeys were correlated with loss of DHA from the brain and retina (8, 12). Similarly, more recent reports stated that human infants incapable of synthesizing DHA (Zellweger syndrome) had reduced brain and retinal DHA concentrations and suffered from blindness and general atonia (13). Some of the symptoms were reversed with DHA supplementation (13).

The theory that human infants may be at risk of n-3 fatty acid or DHA deficiency arose primarily from the rhesus monkey studies, because the n-3 fatty acid–deficient diets used in these studies were similar in fatty acid composition to many commercially available infant formulas of the time (14). Subsequent clinical studies with both preterm and term infants attempted to address the issue of dietary n-3 PUFA requirements for the development of visual and neural function. Our review is based on randomized clinical trials only, because these studies limit the bias and confounding influences associated with comparisons between breast- and formula-fed infants.


RANDOMIZED CONTROLLED TRIALS  
Nine placebo-controlled, randomized studies of formula feeding with different amounts of n-3 PUFA supplementation in term infants have been published (15–22) or reported in an unpublished abstract (U Clausen, A Damli, UV Schenck, B Koletzko, unpublished observations, 1996) (Tables 1 and 2) and many more are in progress. These trials have proved difficult to interpret because different research groups have used diverse dietary sources of PUFAs and the developmental assessments have also differed. Not surprisingly, therefore, some studies have found a beneficial effect of n-3 PUFA supplementation (17, 18, 20, 22), whereas others reported no effect (15, 16, 19, and Clausen et al, unpublished observations, 1996). Before we can decide whether these studies actually help us determine the n-3 PUFA requirements of term infants, we need to examine several issues critically.


View this table:
TABLE 1.. Neurodevelopmental outcomes of term infants fed breast milk or n-3 fatty acid–supplemented formula in randomized clinical trials1  

View this table:
TABLE 2.. Neurodevelopmental outcomes of term infants given long-chain–fatty acid supplementation in randomized clinical trials1  
Dietary treatments
The fatty acid compositions of the standard formulas, particularly the LA to ALA ratio and the absolute amounts of LA and ALA, are important because these determine the rates at which n-6 and n-3 LCPUFAs are synthesized by the infants (24). This issue has important implications for a critical examination of the literature. First, there is the basic question of whether the n-3 requirements can be met by providing ALA alone rather than adding LCPUFAs. Second, how do we interpret the results of various LCPUFA-supplementation studies that used different LA to ALA ratios in the standard formula?

Several studies have examined the effects of various LA-to-ALA ratios by using biochemical endpoints (2, 4) and it is clear that increasing the concentration and proportion of ALA in formula fats improves the DHA status of infants, yet tissue concentrations of DHA do not reach those found in breast-fed infants. Nevertheless, the possibility exists that the highest concentrations of DHA attained may be enough to meet all the nutritional needs of infants. However, few studies have addressed the potential physiologic effects of altering ALA concentrations in formula-fed infants (Table 1). Jensen et al (15) tested LA to ALA ratios ranging from 5:1 to 45:1. Although they measured higher plasma and erythrocyte DHA concentrations in infants fed the lowest-ratio formula (25), the only visual test attempted was VEP latency, which has not previously been reported to be sensitive to dietary manipulation. Innis et al (16) used a more widely accepted test of visual performance (acuity), but the range of LA-to-ALA ratios was small (7–9:1). Both formulas had ALA concentrations >1.9% of total fats and no differences in infant fatty acid profiles or visual acuity were detected.

The ratios of LA to ALA and the concentrations of ALA in standard (control) formulas used in LCPUFA intervention studies have also varied (Table 2). Whether this helps to explain the variation in reported neurologic responses is difficult to ascertain because few groups have used identical testing procedures. In addition, the base formula composition is not always under the control of the investigators. In our own studies, the LA to ALA ratios of reference or control formulas have become lower over the years because of the general trend of manufacturers to increase the concentration of n-3 PUFAs in formulas that has undoubtedly influenced the concentrations of DHA in infant tissues.

The alternative strategy for improving the PUFA blends of infant formulas has been to add LCPUFAs directly. The type of LCPUFA added has largely been governed by the availability of different oils and LCPUFA supplements. For example, at the beginning of our trials the only feasible DHA source was a fish oil high in eicosapentaenoic acid (EPA, 20:5n-3) (Table 2). More recent studies have avoided the addition of EPA by using fish oils high in DHA, such as tuna oil (19), or by supplementing with fractions of egg phospholipids (18–20). Egg phospholipids can be used to mimic the concentrations of DHA and AA found in the breast milk of Western women (18–20), although they provide these fatty acids in a form different from that of breast milk and can contain extra phosphate, choline, and cholesterol (26). Other trials currently underway are using oils from unicellular organisms such as fungi and algae. The toxicology of all LCPUFA sources is rarely reported; therefore, further investigations are needed to evaluate the safety of these substances for consumption by infants.

Power of the study
Calculations of statistical power are used to estimate the sample size that is required to show an effect in a trial. Most randomized trials have enrolled and followed 20–30 infants per treatment group (Tables 1 and 2). Although these numbers are ample for detecting changes in the fatty acid profiles of plasma and erythrocyte membranes and some visual outcomes, they may be inadequate for determining true differences in developmental scores and growth. Studies of adequate power are necessary to determine where differences due to dietary treatments exist and hence to be able to state with confidence which n-3 PUFA concentration is associated with a particular functional outcome. The issue of insufficient sample size in clinical trials has been reviewed elsewhere (27).

Visual and cognitive assessments
There has been some confusion among nutritionists regarding the neural assessments used to determine the potential benefits of n-3 PUFAs during infancy. Many authors have focused on visual outcome measures because these can be objective or easy to use, whereas other researchers have chosen more global assessments of development. The relevance and use of different tests for the assessment of visual and cognitive function in relation to dietary PUFAs have been reviewed extensively by Carlson et al (28). The variety of tests used has added to the complexity of determining specific dietary n-3 fatty acid thresholds for various visual and cognitive functions. For example, visual acuity may be tested by electrophysiologic [visual evoked potential (VEP)] or behavioral methods. Even for just these 2 types of methods, different research groups extrapolated the results differently to determine visual acuity. Some investigators have used VEP to measure latency, which cannot be translated to acuity (15). Similarly, research groups that have opted for more global assessments of development have used tests that measure different aspects of mental, language, and motor development (Table 2). Not surprisingly, all have shown different effects. Studies with similar assessment protocols will be necessary to elucidate the dietary requirement for n-3 PUFAs by using indexes of visual and cognitive development. Because of the use of different assessment methods, caution should be exercised if such studies are combined for meta-analyses.

Confounding influences and effect modifiers
All neural processes are complex and multifactorial. Assessments are subject to various influences despite efforts to exclude bias and confounding through randomization. For example, the requirement may be different for boys compared with girls, or for babies of smoking compared with nonsmoking mothers. This may hold true even for electrophysiologically determined responses. For example, we have detected poorer VEP acuity scores in infants whose mothers smoked than in a comparable group of infants whose mothers did not smoke; there was also a trend toward better acuity scores in female infants (M Neumann, M Makrides, R Gibson, unpublished observations, 1996).

When randomized clinical trials use more global measures of neural development, such as the Bayley's Scales of Infant Development, there are even more potential effect modifiers and confounders that require consideration. To illustrate this point, an examination of the literature relating to comparisons between breast-feeding and formula feeding is instructive and gives some idea of the range of factors that can impinge upon the outcome measure (29–32). Although differences in social class, education, and parenting styles between mothers who choose to breast-feed and those who choose to formula feed would be reduced in a randomized clinical trial by comparing the outcomes of formula-fed infants randomly assigned to receive either a control or an n-3-PUFA-modified formula, noteworthy confounding environmental and biological variables still remain. For example, factors that have all been implicated as affecting developmental scores in populations of infants include sex; race; gestational age; birth weight; incidence of infection or illness; bilirubin levels in the first week of life; alcohol use in pregnancy; strenuous exercise during pregnancy; and maternal parity, smoking, nutrition, weight, age, and marital status (32–34). To control for all known variables, and perhaps some that have yet to be discovered, will require large numbers of subjects, stringent inclusion and exclusion criteria, or both.


SUMMARY  
The human need for dietary PUFAs is unquestioned, but we must accurately define the PUFA requirements of not only infants but also groups that we suspect may have special needs, such as pregnant women and the aged. For example, even for LA there is no known plasma concentration that corresponds to a specific clinical condition in the manner that has been documented for many vitamins and minerals. Instead, a low intake of LA is estimated from the presence of a surrogate marker, mead acid. The absolute requirement for ALA is also unclear, and although dietary recommendations have been made, they have not been correlated with plasma concentrations of either ALA or any of its metabolites. Furthermore, there is no plasma concentration of n-3 LCPUFAs that can be unequivocally related to a physiologic or developmental response in humans.

It seems reasonable to suggest that the PUFA requirements of some groups (eg, pregnant or nonpregnant women, the young or old) could be framed in such a way that dietary intakes would be related to plasma concentrations which in turn would be related to a desired outcome. In some areas, such as vascular disease, there may be sufficient data to be able to relate plasma PUFA concentrations to specific plasma cholesterol concentrations (LDL or HDL subfractions) or a physiologic test such as blood flow, blood pressure, or whole blood or platelet aggregation. However, almost no data are available currently to support such recommendations for infants. Until we have a specific outcome measure (eg, visual acuity, cognitive function scores, insulin sensitivity index, or growth) that we can relate to blood PUFA concentrations (with all dietary fat indexes, ie, the ratios of LA to ALA and of polyunsaturated to saturated fatty acids and intakes of n-6 and n-3 LCPUFAs, defined to reach that concentration), we may need to rely on the composition of breast milk from well-nourished mothers as a guide to dietary recommendations for infants. Because the LCPUFAs of breast milk appear to be dependent on maternal dietary LCPUFAs, it also seems prudent to ensure that breast milk from mothers who include some fish in their diets is used to guide dietary recommendations for infants.


REFERENCES  

  1. British Nutrition Foundation Iron Task Force. Iron: Nutritional and physiological significance. London: Chapman & Hall, 1995:1–180.
  2. Clark KJ, Makrides M, Neumann MA, Gibson RA. Determination of the optimal ratio of linoleic acid to alpha-linolenic acid in infant formulas. J Pediatr 1992;120:S151–8.
  3. Fomon SJ. Fat. In: Craven L, ed. Nutrition of normal infants. 1st ed. St Louis: Mosby, 1993:147–75.
  4. Putnam JC, Carlson SE, DeVoe PW, Barness LA. The effect of variations in dietary fatty acids on the fatty acid composition of erythrocyte phosphatidylcholine and phosphatidylethanolamine in human infants. Am J Clin Nutr 1982;36:106–14.
  5. Aggett PJ, Haschke F, Heine W, et al. Comment on the content and composition of lipids in infant formulas. ESPGAN Committee on Nutrition. Acta Paediatr Scand 1991;80:887–96.
  6. Anderson GJ, Connor WE. On the demonstration of -3 essential-fatty-acid deficiency in humans. Am J Clin Nutr 1989;49:585–7.
  7. Fiennes RN, Sinclair AJ, Crawford MA. Essential fatty acid studies in primates linolenic acid requirements of capuchins. J Med Primatol 1973;2:155–69.
  8. Neuringer M, Connor WE, Van Petten C, Barstad L. Dietary omega-3 fatty acid deficiency and visual loss in infant rhesus monkeys. J Clin Invest 1984;73:272–6.
  9. Holman RT, Johnson SB, Hatch TF. A case of human linolenic acid deficiency involving neurological abnormalities. Am J Clin Nutr 1982;35:617–23.
  10. Stein TP, Marino PL, Harner RN, Schluter MD, Leskiw MJ, Black S. Linoleate and possibly linolenate deficiency in a patient on long-term intravenous nutrition at home. J Am Coll Nutr 1983;2:241–7.
  11. Bjerve KS, Mostad IL, Thoresen L. Alpha-linolenic acid deficiency in patients on long-term gastric-tube feeding: estimation of linolenic acid and long-chain unsaturated n-3 fatty acid requirement in man. Am J Clin Nutr 1987;45:66–77.
  12. Neuringer M, Connor WE, Lin DS, Barstad L, Luck S. Biochemical and functional effects of prenatal and postnatal omega-3 fatty acid deficiency on retina and brain in rhesus monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1986;83:4021–5.
  13. Martinez M. Polyunsaturated fatty acids in the developing human brain, erythrocytes and plasma in peroxisomal disease: therapeutic implications. J Inherit Metab Dis 1995;18:61–75.
  14. Gibson RA, Kneebone GM. Fatty acid composition of infant formulae. Aust Paediatr J 1981;17:46–53.
  15. Jensen CL, Prager TC, Fraley JK, Chen HM, Anderson RE, Heird WC. Effect of dietary linoleic/alpha-linolenic acid ratio on growth and visual function of term infants. J Pediatr 1997;131:200–9.
  16. Innis SM, Akrabawi SS, Diersen-Schade DA, Dobson MV, Guy DG. Visual acuity and blood lipids in term infants fed human milk or formulae. Lipids 1997;32:63–72.
  17. Makrides M, Neumann M, Simmer K, Pater J, Gibson R. Are long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids essential nutrients in infancy? Lancet 1995;345:1463–8.
  18. Carlson SE, Ford AJ, Werkman SH, Peeples JM, Koo WWK. Visual acuity and fatty acid status of term infants fed human milk and formulas with and without docosahexaenoate and arachidonate from egg yolk lecithin. Pediatr Res 1996;39:882–8.
  19. Auestad N, Montalto MB, Hall RT, et al. Visual acuity, erythrocyte fatty acid composition, and growth in term infants fed formulas with long chain polyunsaturated fatty acids for one year. Pediatr Res 1997;41:1–10.
  20. Agostoni C, Trojan S, Bellù R, Riva E, Giovannini M. Neurodevelopmental quotient of healthy term infants at 4 months and feeding practice: the role of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids. Pediatr Res 1995;38:262–6.
  21. Jorgensen MH, Holmer G, Lund P, Hernell O, Michaelsen, KF. Effect of formula supplemented with docosahexaenoic acid and gamma-linolenic acid on fatty acid status and visual acuity in term infants. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 1998;26:412–21.
  22. Birch EE, Hoffman DR, Uauy R, Birch DG, Prestidge C. Visual acuity and the essentiality of docosahexaenoic acid and arachidonic acid in the diet of term infants. Pediatr Res 1998;44:201–9.
  23. Janowsky JS, Scott DT, Wheeler RE, Auestad N. Fatty acids affect early language development. Pediatr Res 1995;37:310A (abstr).
  24. Gibson RA, Makrides M, Neumann MA, Simmer K, Mantzioris E, James MJ. Ratios of linoleic acid to -linolenic acid in formulas for term infants. J Pediatr 1994;125:S48–55.
  25. Jensen CL, Chen H, Fraley JK, Anderson RE, Heird WC. Biochemical effects of dietary linoleic/alpha-linolenic acid ratio in term infants. Lipids 1996;31:107–13.
  26. Agostoni C, Riva E, Bellù R, Trojan S, Luotti D, Giovannini M. Effects of diet on the lipid and fatty acid status of full-term infants at 4 months. J Am Coll Nutr 1994;13:658–64.
  27. Moher D, Dulberg CS, Wells GA. Statistical power, sample size, and their reporting in randomized controlled trials. JAMA 1994; 272:122–4.
  28. Carlson SE, Neuringer M, Reisbick S. Assessment of infant visual and cognitive function in relation to long chain polyunsaturated fatty acids. 1st ed. Basel, Switzerland: Editiones Roche, 1996:1–78.
  29. Taylor B, Wadsworth J. Breast feeding and child development at five years. Dev Med Child Neurol 1984;26:73–80.
  30. Rogan WJ, Gladen BC. Breast-feeding and cognitive development. Early Hum Dev 1993;31:181–93.
  31. Morrow-Tlucak M, Haude RH, Ernhart CB. Breastfeeding and cognitive development in the first 2 years of life. Soc Sci Med 1988; 26:635–9.
  32. Silva PA, Buckfield P, Spears GF. Some maternal and child developmental characteristics associated with breast feeding: a report from the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Child Development Study. Aust Paediatr J 1978;14:265–8.
  33. Lucas A, Morley R, Cole TJ, Lister G, Leeson-Payne C. Breast milk and subsequent intelligence quotient in children born preterm. Lancet 1992;339:261–4.
  34. Chen Y-J, Kang W-M. Effects of bilirubin on visual evoked potentials in term infants. Eur J Pediatr 1995;154:662–6.

作者: Robert A Gibson
医学百科App—中西医基础知识学习工具
  • 相关内容
  • 近期更新
  • 热文榜
  • 医学百科App—健康测试工具