Literature
首页医源资料库在线期刊美国临床营养学杂志2000年71卷第5期

Sociocultural and behavioral influences on weight gain during pregnancy

来源:《美国临床营养学杂志》
摘要:ABSTRACTStudieshaveconsistentlyidentifiedapositiveassociationbetweenprenatalweightgainandbirthweight。Muchless,however,isknownaboutfactorsthatmayinfluencewomentogainweightwithincurrentlyrecommendedranges。40%ofwomenactuallygainweightwithintheseranges。This......

点击显示 收起

Carol A Hickey

1 From the Department of Maternal and Child Health, School of Public Health, University of Alabama at Birmingham.

2 Presented at the symposium Maternal Nutrition: New Developments and Implications, held in Paris, June 11–12, 1998.

3 Supported by Maternal and Child Health grant MCJ-9040, Center for Research and Information on Nutrition, Paris.

4 Reprints not available. Address correspondence to CA Hickey, 320 Ryals Public Health Building, 1665 University Boulevard, Birmingham, AL 35294-0022. E-mail: hickeyc{at}uab.edu.


ABSTRACT  
Studies have consistently identified a positive association between prenatal weight gain and birth weight. Much less, however, is known about factors that may influence women to gain weight within currently recommended ranges. The importance of this issue is suggested by recent reports indicating that only 30–40% of women actually gain weight within these ranges. This paper examines demographic, sociocultural, and behavioral factors that are associated with, and may influence risk of, low prenatal weight gain among adult women with low and normal body mass indexes. Available data suggest that these factors include ethnicity, socioeconomic status, age, education, pregnancy intendedness or wantedness, prenatal advice, and psychosocial characteristics such as attitude toward weight gain, social support, depression, stress, anxiety, and self-efficacy. Potential theoretical models for these associations include biological, behavioral, and mixed pathways. The design of targeted intervention studies will depend on further identification and characterization of sociocultural and behavioral risk factors that, along with reproductive and nutritional characteristics, may predict which women are most likely to have inadequate prenatal weight gain.

Key Words: Prenatal weight gain • demographics • socioeconomics • culture • psychosocial risk factors • behavioral risk factors • pregnancy weight gain • birth weight


INTRODUCTION  
Studies have consistently identified a positive association between prenatal weight gain and birth weight (1). Studies published in the past 10 y (2–9) have validated the 1990 Institute of Medicine (IOM) guidelines for weight gain during pregnancy (1), which are based on pregravid body mass index (BMI; in kg/m2). Prenatal weight gain within the suggested range for each pregravid BMI category is associated with more favorable outcomes than is weight gain above or below the suggested range. These outcomes include a reduction in the prevalence of low-birth-weight infants (<2500 g) (6), small-for-gestational age infants (2, 3), large-for-gestational age infants (2), high-birth-weight infants (>4500 g) (6), cesarean deliveries (2), and preterm deliveries (4, 5), as well as an increase in mean birth weight (3, 7).

Much less, however, is known about factors that may influence women to gain or not gain weight within the recommended ranges. The importance of this issue is suggested by reports (8–14) indicating that only 30–40% of women actually gain weight within these ranges during their pregnancies. This paper examines recent reports of demographic, sociocultural, and behavioral factors that are associated with, and may influence the risk of, low prenatal weight gain among adult women of low and normal BMI who deliver at term. Low weight gain is defined by using the lower limit of the IOM range when possible (<11.5 kg for BMIs <19.8 and <12.5 kg for BMIs from 19.8 to 26) or the cutoff point specified in the study being reviewed.


DEMOGRAPHIC CHARACTERISTICS OF WOMEN WITH LOW PRENATAL WEIGHT GAIN  
The 1989 revision of the US Standard Certificate of Live Birth included prenatal weight gain for the first time (15). In 1995, the District of Columbia and all states except California included this item on their birth certificates, representing 86% of all births in the United States (16). Unfortunately, this weight gain information is based on the answer to a single question about the amount of "weight gained during pregnancy" in pounds; pregravid weight and height data are not collected. These data, as published, can only be examined in terms of the incidence of prenatal weight gain <16 lb (<7.3 kg), an amount that would be considered inadequate regardless of pregravid BMI. Other national sources of information on prenatal weight gain include the 1988 National Maternal and Infant Health Survey (NMIHS; 10, 17), the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) Pregnancy Nutrition Surveillance System (PNSS; 18, 19), and the Pregnancy Risk Assessment Monitoring System (20). These data sets do include pregravid weight and height.

Ethnic origin
US birth certificate data for 1995 indicate that 10.4% and 9.3%, respectively, of women completing 37–39 wk and 40 wk of gestation gained <7.3 kg (16). The incidence of prenatal weight gain <7.3 kg for these gestational lengths was highest among non-Hispanic black women (15.5% and 14.0%, respectively), followed by Hispanic women (12.4% and 11.2%, respectively) and non-Hispanic white women (8.8% and 8.0%, respectively). Among Hispanic women delivering at 40 wk gestation, the incidence of prenatal weight gain <7.3 kg was highest for women of Mexican origin (13.6%), followed by women of Puerto Rican (12.5%), Central and South American (10.6%), and Cuban (6.9%) origin. Substantial differences in the incidence of low weight gain have also been reported for women in other ethnic groups. Disregarding gestational length, 14.8% of American Indian, 11.2% of other Asian or Pacific Islander, 9.9% of Japanese, 8.2% of Hawaiian, 7.4% of Filipino, and 6.3% of Chinese women gained <7.3 kg prenatally in 1995 (16).

The 1988 NMIHS was a systematic sample (by race, age, marital status, and birth weight strata) of women who had live births in the United States in 1988 (10). Women were interviewed between 10 and 18 mo after delivery; information was obtained on pregravid weight, height, and weight gain during pregnancy. The NMIHS estimates are representative of 1322820 white and 205507 black women aged 15 y with BMIs 29 who delivered singleton, term infants (37 wk gestation). Half (50.0%) of underweight black women (BMI < 19.8) and more than one-third (35.7%) of underweight white women reported prenatal weight gain that would be considered low (<12.5 kg) according to IOM guidelines. Similarly, 36.1% of black women and 24.7% of white women with average BMIs reported low weight gain (<11.5 kg). These differences in the incidence of low weight gain among black and white women have been confirmed by 3 other studies (3, 11, 21) as well as by 1993 CDC PNSS data (18, 19).

Siega-Riz and Hobel (14) reported that 26.9% of 4791 nonobese (BMI < 26.0) Hispanic women had poor prenatal weight gain (<9.5 kg). These women were predominantly low-income, recent Mexican immigrants attending public health clinics in West Los Angeles. The CDC (12) used data from the PNSS to examine prenatal weight gain in 4840 migrant farm workers in 4 eastern states during 1989–1993. Most of the farm workers (58.4%) were Hispanic; 28.7% were white, non-Hispanic; and 11.1% were black, non-Hispanic (1.8% were other). More than half (52%) gained less than the IOM guidelines for their BMI, compared with 31.6% of 610728 nonmigrant women. All were enrolled in the Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children (WIC). The US work force includes an estimated 3–5 million migrant and seasonal farm workers and 16% of these are women (12). Although these data may not represent the total pregnant migrant worker population because data were not available from western states or from women not enrolled in the WIC program, they suggest that migrant workers may also be a group at high risk for low prenatal weight gain.

Socioeconomic status
Data from the 1980 National Natality Survey (21) indicated that the risk of low weight gain (<7.3 kg) increased nearly 2-fold as annual household income fell from $30000 (9.0% low weight gain) to <$9000 (15.9% low weight gain). More recent nationally representative data by income category are not available. However, the CDC PNSS provides data for low-income women who participate in publicly funded health, nutrition, and food assistance programs in 22 states, the District of Columbia, the Navajo Nations, Puerto Rico, and American Samoa (18, 19). The system is limited by variation in data quality and completeness, as well as by the fact that it is a convenience rather than a random sample of low-income women (18, 19). However, in 1993, one-third (33%) of the women included in the PNSS experienced low weight gain, as defined by the lower limit of the IOM guidelines.

Maternal age and parity
Prenatal weight gain has also been linked to maternal age and parity. Primiparas have been reported to be less likely to have low weight gain (<6.8 kg) than multiparas, but little difference has been observed by age or parity among multiparas (17). Analyses of 1994 data from the CDC PNSS used the 1990 IOM guidelines to evaluate pregnancy weight gain by pregravid BMI categories (19). Women aged 30 y were the most likely to gain less than the IOM guidelines and the least likely to gain more.

Maternal education and marital status
US national data for 1992 indicate that weight gain increases with educational attainment; gains of <7.3 kg are nearly 3 times as common (14%) for women with less than an elementary school education as for women with 16 y of schooling (5%) (22). Unmarried mothers are more likely than married mothers to gain <7.3 kg (13% compared with 8%).

Multivariate analyses of demographic characteristics
Kleinman (17) used 1980 National Natality Survey data on maternal ethnicity, age, parity, and education for married mothers with live singleton births in a logistic regression model with low weight gain (<6.8 kg) as the dependent variable. (The model also included smoking, alcohol consumption, height, and pregravid BMI.) Black women had a 71% greater risk and Hispanic women a 100% greater risk of low weight gain than white women. Mothers with <12 y of education had a 63% greater risk and mothers with 12 y of education had a 26% greater risk of low weight gain than mothers with >12 y of education. Primiparas were less likely to have low weight gain than multiparas but there was little difference by age or parity among multiparas. Overall, however, the variables in this analysis accounted for <5% of the variance in maternal weight gain.

Caulfield et al (11) reported that only 28.2% of 2617 black and 32.5% of 1253 white women with singleton pregnancies of 28-wk duration experienced prenatal weight gain within the ranges suggested by the IOM guidelines. Black women were 1.51 (95% CI: 1.23, 1.85) times more likely to undergain and 0.89 (95% CI: 0.74, 1.08) times less likely to overgain than white women (adjusted odds ratios). Multinomial regression analysis identified maternal pregravid BMI, height, parity, education, smoking, blood pressure, race, length of pregnancy, and sex of the fetus as risk factors for under- or overgain in this population. Maternal age, diabetes status, and provider type (managed care, hospital obstetrical department, or private practice) were not statistically significant and were dropped from the final model. No interactions were found between any factor examined and BMI or race.

Siega-Riz and Hobel (14), in a study of 2988 underweight and normal-weight (BMI <26) Hispanic women, found that only short stature (<157 cm) was associated with an increased adjusted odds ratio (AOR: 1.5; 95% CI: 1.24, 1.84) for low weight gain (<9.5 kg). Factors associated with a decreased risk of low weight gain included being US born, being primiparous and <29 y of age, planning the pregnancy, and having a close relative die during the pregnancy.

Finally, note that a study by Abrams et al (23) suggested that when rates or patterns of maternal gain (rather than total gain) are considered, the effect of individual sociodemographic factors (ie, race or ethnicity, age, and parity) may vary by trimester. The most important maternal predictors of first trimester weight gain were age (negative association) and Asian race or ethnicity (negative association). During the second trimester, pregravid body mass was most predictive (negative association), followed by parity (negative association) and black race or ethnicity (negative association). In the third trimester, maternal hypertension (positive association), age (negative association), and parity (negative association) were the most important predictors.


BEHAVIORAL CHARACTERISTICS OF WOMEN WITH LOW WEIGHT GAIN  
Smoking and alcohol use
Kleinman (17) used a logistic regression model to analyze 1980 National Natality Survey data on smoking and alcohol consumption for married mothers with live singleton births with low weight gain (<6.8 kg) as the dependent variable. As noted above, the model also included ethnicity, age, parity, education, height, and pregravid BMI. Smokers had a 46% greater risk of low weight gain than did nonsmokers. Mothers reporting light alcohol consumption (2 drinks less than once a month) had a 24% smaller risk for low weight gain and mothers reporting moderate alcohol consumption (those who drank more than once a month or >2 drinks when they drank) had a 47% smaller risk of low weight gain relative to nondrinkers. A more recent study did not find differences in prenatal weight gain among mothers consuming different amounts of alcohol compared with nondrinkers (24). Data from the 1995 Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System indicated that 16.3% of pregnant women surveyed reported consuming at least one drink during the previous month (25). Approximately 3.5% reported frequent drinking (consumption of an average of 7 drinks/wk or 5 drinks on at least one occasion), which placed their infants at risk for adverse effects, including fetal alcohol syndrome, regardless of prenatal weight gain (25).

An estimated 26% of US women of reproductive age (18–44 y) smoked in 1993, and 19–27% of women smoked during pregnancy (26). Maternal smoking appears to cause fetal growth restriction through both nonnutritional and nutritional routes, and the results of numerous studies suggest that smoking and maternal weight gain during pregnancy are independent, additive predictors of birth weight (1, 27). Muscati et al (28) estimated that the etiologic fraction of fetal growth restriction attributable to smoking is 30.8%, whereas 16.7% is attributable to low pregravid weight of the mother, and 15.3% is attributable to low prenatal weight gain. In another study of smoking and weight change during pregnancy, 36.4% of normal-weight women (BMI: 19.8–26) and 41.7% of obese women (BMI >29) who smoked were observed to have prenatal weight gain below the IOM guidelines, compared with 23.2% and 36.0%, respectively, of normal-weight and obese nonsmokers (29). Mongoven et al (30) reported that white, non-Hispanic pregnant smokers (5 cigarettes/wk) who quit smoking before 28 wk gestation were half as likely to have total weight gains below the IOM guidelines [relative risk (RR): 0.47; 95% CI: 0.27, 0.81] as women who continued to smoke. They were also 1.74 times more likely to have weight gains above the IOM recommendations (95% CI: 1.21, 2.51).

Pregnancy intendedness and wantedness
Reports on the association of pregnancy intendedness or wantedness with subsequent maternal weight gain are few and mixed. Among 536 nonobese (BMI 26.0) low-income black women, having a mistimed but wanted or having an unwanted pregnancy was associated with a doubling of the AOR (2.0; 95% CI: 1.2, 3.2) for low weight gain (10 kg) compared with wanted pregnancies (31). A similar association was not observed among 270 white women (AOR: 1.0; 95% CI: 0.6, 1.9) in the same study. Among 4245 Hispanic women (BMI < 26), having a planned compared with an unplanned pregnancy was associated with a decreased AOR (0.82; 95% CI: 0.67, 1.00) for low weight gain (<9.5 kg), although the 95% CI included 1.0 (14). Conversely, in a multivariate examination of first pregnancies in 6015 relatively young (aged 19–27 y) black, Hispanic, and white primiparous women included in the National Longitudinal Survey of Labor Market Experiences of Youth (32), pregnancy wantedness was not a significant predictor of very low prenatal weight gain (defined as 6.8 kg regardless of BMI).

More recently, Kost et al (33) completed multivariate analyses of data from the 1988 NMIHS and the 1988 National Survey of Family Growth to determine whether women with unplanned births differed from other women in their pregnancy behaviors, independent of their social and demographic characteristics. Women with intended conceptions were more likely than similar women with unintended pregnancies to recognize early signs of pregnancy and to seek out early prenatal care and were somewhat more likely to quit smoking. However, they were not more likely than women with comparable social and demographic characteristics to adhere to a recommended schedule of prenatal visits once they began care, to reduce alcohol intake, or to follow their clinician's advice about taking vitamins and gaining weight.

Response to weight gain advice
The US Public Health Service Expert Panel on the Content of Prenatal Care issued a report in 1989 that made specific recommendations regarding the procedures and advice to be included within the context of prenatal care (34). The Expert Panel recommended that pregnant women receive advice in 7 areas: 1) breast-feeding; 2) reducing or eliminating alcohol use; 3) reducing or eliminating smoking; 4) not using illegal drugs such as marijuana, cocaine, or crack; 5) eating the proper foods during pregnancy; 6) taking vitamin or mineral supplements; and 7) gaining an appropriate amount of weight during pregnancy. Subsequent studies examined the influence of the Expert Panel's recommendations on birth outcomes (35–37). However, the influence of specific and documented weight gain advice on observed prenatal weight gain in US women of known pregravid BMI categories has not been examined in prospective randomized, controlled intervention trials.

Differences in weight gain have been reported for the intervention and control groups in several US and Canadian studies of women with unspecified pregravid BMI in which pregnancy outcome, rather than maternal weight gain, was the primary outcome variable (38–42). The interventions in these studies included nutrition advice alone (39) or such advice linked with other interventions, including home visits by nutritionists (38), supplemental food (38, 41), a nurse home visitation program (40), and the provision of prenatal care through multidisciplinary rather than traditional clinics (42). Although the positive direction of the difference reported for mean weight gain in each of the studies favored the intervention group, in 3 of the 5 studies these differences were not statistically significant (38, 40, 42). In the other 2 studies (39, 41), differences between the control and intervention groups in the time periods (gestational weeks) between baseline and final weight observations could have accounted for a considerable proportion of the increased weight gain reported for the intervention groups. Additionally, because these studies were completed before release of the IOM guidelines (1), only mean weight gain was reported; no attempts were made to assess weight gain by using a range or cutoff point based on pregravid BMI.

Brown et al (43) developed a prenatal weight gain intervention program based on social marketing methods, which included focus groups among prenatal clinic patients before the design of the intervention. The program was implemented in 2 public health clinics and a third clinic served as a control site (44). Unfortunately, several unexpected circumstances arose during the study (44) that hampered an evaluation of the intervention; these included a decline in clinic enrollment and changes in the control clinic protocol to emphasize greater weight gain. However, preliminary evidence indicated that prenatal weight gain and birth weights of African Americans in the intervention group did not differ significantly from those of whites, whereas both weight gain and birth weight were significantly lower in African Americans than in whites in the control group (44).

A report by Taffel et al (45), based on analysis of data from the 1988 NMIHS, indicated that only 12% of white mothers reported receiving advice regarding prenatal weight gain that did not meet the minimum standard in effect in 1988 (10 kg), and only 19% reported receiving advice that did not meet the minimum IOM recommendation (1) for their pregravid BMI. In contrast, a significantly higher proportion of black mothers reported receiving advice to gain <10 kg (33%) or less than the lower limit of the IOM guidelines (34%). The higher frequency of inappropriate advice reported by black mothers could not be explained by differences in pregravid BMI, age, education, parity, marital status, or site of care. Nevertheless, because apparent compliance with the perceived advice was almost the same for black and white mothers (>70% gained 10 kg when this was the reported weight-gain advice), Taffel et al suggested that it is entirely feasible that more appropriate weight-gain advice for black women would result in more appropriate weight gain.

Although the combined evidence from the above studies suggests a potential relation between prenatal nutrition advice and prenatal weight gain, the many threats to the validity of inference in these, as well as other studies of the specific components of prenatal care, need appraisal (46). These include, in addition to differences in pregravid nutritional status and BMI, issues such as self-selection bias, recall bias, differences in time during gestation when nutrition advice was given, variation in content and frequency of advice, the pairing of advice with other food or nonfood interventions, individual and social characteristics of the provider as contrasted with those of the pregnant woman, and disparities in weight gain advice given to women from different ethnic (45) and socioeconomic (47) groups.


BELIEFS, ATTITUDES, AND KNOWLEDGE OF WOMEN WITH LOW PRENATAL WEIGHT GAIN  
There have been many studies of dietary intake during pregnancy; some of them have examined the influence of beliefs, attitudes, and knowledge on eating behavior and dietary intake. These studies will not be reviewed because they do not provide information on prenatal weight gain. Several studies have examined beliefs, attitudes, and knowledge related to body weight or weight gain in pregnancy (48–53). Three (51–53) of the latter have examined the association between attitude toward gain and actual weight gain during pregnancy. Only one study (53) tested the association between attitude toward gain and low prenatal weight gain in women with documented pregravid BMI or weight-for-height categories. None have presented analyses by ethnic group.

Palmer et al (51) developed a Likert-format questionnaire with 18 statements expressing attitudes of pregnant women toward their own prenatal weight gain. Positive attitude scores among 29 white, mainly middle-class women with at least a high-school education were reported to be significantly associated with higher actual weight gain (P < 0.025). Stevens-Simon et al (52) used the same Palmer scale (51) in a study of 99 ethnically diverse, pregnant 13–18-y-olds and reported that weight gain was significantly related to 4 of the 18 scale items but not to the total attitude scale score. Copper et al (53) administered the Palmer scale (51) to 1000 black (69%) and white (31%) adult, low-income pregnant women and reported that the attitude score was not significantly related to prenatal weight gain (r = -0.05, P = 0.08). Maternal attitude toward weight gain was found to be influenced by pregravid body size; thin women tended to have positive attitudes and obese women tended to have negative attitudes about weight gain [as was the case with adolescents in the study of Stevens-Simon et al (52)]. However, within BMI groups, a positive attitude did not predict appropriate weight gain (defined 10 kg). No significant differences in attitude scores were reported between black and white women in any pregravid BMI category; data for the association between attitude score and weight gain were not reported separately by ethnic group.

The mixed and conflicting results from studies using the Palmer scale (51) to examine attitudes toward prenatal weight gain are not unexpected, given that the scale was originally developed to examine attitudes among women with entirely different sociodemographic and cultural characteristics, ie, a small group of middle class white women. McLean et al (54) observed that successful tests of etiologic hypotheses and the development of targeted interventions to address the excess rates of adverse pregnancy outcomes among black women depend critically on valid, reliable measures that are specific to black women. This generalization would seem to hold true for low weight gain and for women in other sociodemographic categories as well and serves to underscore the need for community-based ethnographic examinations of low prenatal weight gain that are specific to the defined target population. A careful reading of the studies cited in this paper shows that, with the exception of the study by Brown et al (43, 44), all either took place in research clinic settings removed from the community or were based solely on the analysis of existing data sets. Rarely have women with low prenatal weight gain or the community-based health care providers with whom they interact been actively involved as collaborators or as co-investigators. In general, hypotheses have been imposed from the literature rather than being generated through community-based observations or by the community members themselves. Rarely, if ever, have the data gathered, the observations made, and the hypotheses generated or tested been made available for comment or validation by the women involved as a part of the research process.


CULTURAL INFLUENCES ON PRENATAL WEIGHT GAIN  
More than 10 y ago, Pelto (55) completed an assessment of available studies targeting cultural issues in maternal and child health and nutrition. Her challenge for social scientists remains true today, ie, we have yet to thoroughly and empirically examine cultural and behavioral adaptations to the changed energy and nutrient requirements of pregnancy. Although the literature in anthropology and sociology contains numerous examples of the dietary cravings and food proscriptions attributed to pregnant women, these have almost always been discussed from a symbolic, psychological, or ethnoscientific perspective, rather than in a manner that would allow an assessment of their nutritional consequences (56).

Sociocultural dimensions that may provide a framework for examining cultural issues related to prenatal weight gain include gathering information on ideologic (belief) structures related to appropriate food intake, food proscriptions, activity patterns, weight gain, and body image during pregnancy, as well as the relation of these to household and community characteristics. Concomitantly, this information must be paired with data on observed patterns of behavioral adjustment, energy expenditure, and weight gain. Finally, both types of data may need to be stratified at the subcultural level, even within what at first appear to be fairly homogeneous ethnic and socioeconomic groups.


PSYCHOSOCIAL CHARACTERISTICS OF WOMEN WITH LOW PRENATAL WEIGHT GAIN  
A separate but related area of inquiry has suggested that maternal psychosocial characteristics may influence prenatal weight gain. During the past 15 y, studies have explored the associations of social support (14, 57–59), depression (14, 58, 60, 61), stress (14, 58, 61, 62, 63), trait anxiety (58), mastery (58), and self-esteem (58) with prenatal weight gain. In each case, the results of these preliminary studies were mixed, depending on the assessment instrument used, the population studied, and the type of statistical analysis used (bivariate or multivariate). Additionally, this area of research is complicated by cross-cultural and subcultural differences in the perception and meaning of individual psychosocial characteristics or states, and thus by the related issue of culturally appropriate assessment measures (64, 65) that are suitable for use in the clinical setting (66, 67). For example, likely explanations for the lack of association between maternal psychosocial status and low prenatal weight gain among black compared with white women (58) may involve the following: 1) different perceptions of what constitutes psychosocial stress (65), ie, perceptions not tapped by the scales used in the study; 2) variation in biological and behavioral responses to specific stressors (65); 3) variation in the availability of stress mediators (65, 68); or 4) the existence of entirely separate mechanisms for low prenatal weight gain among black women. Examples of the latter would include differences in the nutrition-related content of prenatal care provided to black women (35, 45).

If these preliminary studies of psychosocial status and prenatal weight gain are confirmed, an examination of the overall literature related to psychosocial determinants of pregnancy outcome suggests 2 theoretical pathways for the observed association with weight gain. Maternal psychosocial stress may function to affect pregnancy outcome through neuroendocrine-mediated alterations in the complex physiologic responses to pregnancy (69, 70). These alterations could also affect pregnancy-related adjustments in basal and resting metabolism and the efficiency with which energy is used to synthesize new tissue, making it more difficult for women to achieve the positive energy balance necessary for gain in maternal and fetal tissue. Alternatively, and perhaps concurrently with possible neuroendocrine-mediated alterations in prenatal energy metabolism, poor psychosocial status may interfere with the achievement of a positive energy balance through stress-related changes in sleep patterns, physical activity, appetite, food intake, tobacco use, or other behaviors. These potential associations have not been examined in studies reported to date.

The study by Picone et al (62, 71) provides support for a hypothesized catecholamine-mediated effect of stress on prenatal energy metabolism. In that study, psychosocial stress (assessed by using an abbreviated Holmes-Rahe life events questionnaire; 72) was correlated negatively with prenatal weight gain (r = -0.35, P < 0.01) but not with energy intake, suggesting that the utilization of dietary energy was less efficient in women experiencing stress. Caan and Petitti (73), in a comment on the Picone study, calculated that "nonsmokers in the low weight gain group consumed a mean of 1617 cal/day to gain a mean of 5.4 lb in their pregnancy (299 cal/day/lb), whereas nonsmokers in the average weight gain group consumed a mean of 1905 cal/day to gain a mean of 32.7 lb (58 cal/day/lb)." This suggested that differences in both energy intake and energy utilization were associated with variations in psychosocial stress.


CONCLUSION  
The studies cited above, when considered as a group, suggest the following: 1) Low prenatal weight gain, despite its documented association with poor pregnancy outcomes, continues to be a problem for a significant proportion of under- and average-weight women enrolled in a variety of health, nutrition education, and food assistance programs. 2) In the United States, the incidence of low weight gain varies considerably by ethnic group, education, marital status, age, parity, smoking status, and pregnancy planning status. 3) Certain psychosocial characteristics (depression, trait anxiety, and low levels of mastery and self-esteem) may be associated with ethnic group–specific increases in the incidence of low weight gain. 4) There are no published prospective, randomized controlled trials of nutritional or other interventions targeting low prenatal weight gain per se (rather than pregnancy outcome) among US women with low and average pregravid BMIs. 5) The design of such intervention trials would be enhanced if informed by the community-based identification and characterization of risk factors specific to the intended target groups (ie, by community-based hypothesis generation and refinement). 6) Low prenatal weight gain may be more amenable to analysis and to intervention if it is conceptualized in terms of a biopsychosocial model (74) that explicitly recognizes the individual and interacting influences of biomedical, psychosocial, and lifestyle factors.


REFERENCES  

  1. Subcommittee on Nutritional Status and Weight Gain During Pregnancy, Institute of Medicine. Nutrition during pregnancy. Washington, DC: National Academy Press, 1990.
  2. Parker JD, Abrams B. Prenatal weight gain advice: an examination of the recent prenatal weight gain recommendations of the Institute of Medicine. Obstet Gynecol 1992;79:664–9.
  3. Hickey CA, Cliver SP, Goldenberg RL, Kohatsu J, Hoffman HJ. Prenatal weight gain, term birth weight, and fetal growth retardation among high-risk multiparous black and white women. Obstet Gynecol 1993;81:529–35.
  4. Siega-Riz AM, Adair LS, Hobel CJ. Institute of Medicine maternal weight gain recommendations and pregnancy outcomes in a predominantly Hispanic population. Obstet Gynecol 1994;84:565–73.
  5. Hickey CA, Cliver SP, McNeal SF, Hoffman HJ, Goldenberg RL. Prenatal weight gain patterns and spontaneous preterm birth among nonobese black and white women. Obstet Gynecol 1995;85:909–14.
  6. Cogswell MA, Serdula MK, Hungerford DW, Yip R. Gestational weight gain among average-weight and overweight women—what is excessive? Am J Obstet Gynecol 1995;172:705–12.
  7. Hickey CA, Cliver SP, McNeal SF, Hoffman HJ, Goldenberg RL. Prenatal weight gain patterns and birth weight among nonobese black and white women. Obstet Gynecol 1996;88:490–6.
  8. Lederman SA, Paxton A, Heymsfield SB, Wang J, Thornton J, Pierson RN. Body fat and water changes during pregnancy in women with different body weight and weight gain. Obstet Gynecol 1997;90:483–8.
  9. Carmichael S, Abrams B, Selvin S. The pattern of maternal weight gain in women with good pregnancy outcome. Am J Public Health 1997;87:1984–8.
  10. Keppel KG, Taffel SM. Pregnancy-related weight gain and retention: implications of the 1990 Institute of Medicine guidelines. Am J Public Health 1993;83:1100–3.
  11. Caulfield LE, Witter FR, Stoltzfus RJ. Determinants of gestational weight gain outside the recommended ranges among black and white women. Obstet Gynecol 1996;87:760–6.
  12. Centers for Disease Control. Pregnancy-related behaviors among migrant farm workers—four states, 1989–1993. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep 1997;46:283–6.
  13. Hickey CA, McNeal SF, Menefee L, Ivey S. Prenatal weight gain within upper and lower recommended ranges: effect on birth weight of black and white infants. Obstet Gynecol 1997;90:489–94.
  14. Siega-Riz AM, Hobel CJ. Predictors of poor maternal weight gain from baseline anthropometric, psychosocial and demographic information in a Hispanic population. J Am Diet Assoc 1997;97:1264–8.
  15. Centers for Disease Control. Pregnancy risks determined from birth certificate data—United States, 1989. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep 1992;41:556–63.
  16. Ventura SJ, Martin JA, Curtin SC, Mathews TJ. Report of final natality statistics, 1995. Monthly vital statistics report. Vol 45. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics, 1997. (no. 11, suppl.)
  17. Kleinman JC. National Center for Health Statistics. Maternal weight gain during pregnancy: determinants and consequences. Hyattsville, MD: Public Health Service, 1990. (NCHS working paper series no. 33.)
  18. Centers for Disease Control. Pregnancy nutrition surveillance system—United States, 1979–1990. MMWR CDC Surveill Summ 1992;41:25–41.
  19. Perry GS, Yip R, Zyrkowski C. Nutritional risk factors among low-income pregnant U.S. women: the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) Pregnancy Nutrition Surveillance System, 1979–1993. Semin Perinatol 1995;19:211–21.
  20. Adams MM, Shulman HB, Bruce C, Hogue C, Brogan D, The PRAMS Working Group. The Pregnancy Risk Assessment Monitoring System: design, questionnaire, data collection and response rate. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol 1991;5:333–46.
  21. Taffel S. National Center for Health Statistics. Maternal weight gain and the outcome of pregnancy, United States, 1980. Vital Health Stat 21 1986;44. [DHHS publication no. (PHS) 86-1922.]
  22. Ventura SJ, Martin JA, Taffel SM, Mathews TJ, Clarke SC. Advance report of final natality statistics, 1992. Monthly vital statistics report. Vol 43. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics, 1994. (no. 5, suppl.)
  23. Abrams B, Carmichael S, Selvin S. Factors associated with the pattern of maternal weight gain during pregnancy. Obstet Gynecol 1995;86:170–6.
  24. Lazzaroni F, Bonassi S, Magnani M, et al. Moderate maternal drinking and outcome of pregnancy. Eur J Epidemiol 1993;9:599–606.
  25. Centers for Disease Control. Alcohol consumption among pregnant and childbearing-aged women—United States, 1991 and 1995. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep 1997;46:346–50.
  26. Centers for Disease Control. Medical-care expenditures attributable to cigarette smoking during pregnancy—United States, 1995. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep 1997;46:1048–50.
  27. Rasmussen KM, Abrams B. Annotation: cigarette smoking, nutrition, and birthweight. Am J Public Health 1997;87:543–6.
  28. Muscati SK, Koski KG, Gray-Donald K. Increased energy intake in pregnant smokers does not prevent human fetal growth retardation. J Nutr 1996;126:2984–9.
  29. Hellerstedt WL, Himes JH, Story M, Alton IR, Edwards LE. The effects of cigarette smoking and gestational weight change on birth outcomes in obese and normal-weight women. Am J Public Health 1997;87:591–6.
  30. Mongoven M, Dolan-Mullen P, Groff JY, Nicol L, Burau K. Weight gain associated with prenatal smoking cessation in white non-Hispanic women. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1996;174:72–7.
  31. Hickey CA, Cliver SP, Goldenberg RL, McNeal SF, Hoffman HJ. Low prenatal weight gain among low-income women: what are the risk factors? Birth 1997;2:102–8.
  32. Marsiglio W, Mott FL. Does wanting to become pregnant with a first child affect subsequent maternal behaviors and infant birth weight? J Marriage Family 1988;50:1023–36.
  33. Kost K, Landry DJ, Darroch JE. Predicting maternal behaviors during pregnancy: does intention status matter? Fam Plann Perspect 1998;30:79–88.
  34. Public Health Service Expert Panel on the Content of Prenatal Care. Caring for our future: the content of prenatal care. Washington, DC: Public Health Service, 1989.
  35. Kogan MD, Alexander GR, Kotelchuck M, Nagey DA, Jack BW. Comparing mothers' reports on the content of prenatal care received with recommended national guidelines for care. Public Health Rep 1994;109:637–46.
  36. Kogan MD, Alexander GR, Kotelchuck M, Nagey D. Relation of the content of prenatal care to the risk of low birth weight: maternal reports of health behavior advice and initial prenatal care procedures. JAMA 1994;271:1340–5.
  37. Sable MR, Herman AA. The relationship between prenatal health behavior advice and low birth weight. Public Health Rep 1997; 112:332–9.
  38. Rush D. Nutritional services during pregnancy and birth weight: a retrospective matched pair analysis. Can Med Assoc J 1981;125:567–76.
  39. Orstead C, Arrington D, Kamath SK, Olson R, Kohrs MB. Efficacy of prenatal nutrition counseling: weight gain, infant birth weight, and cost-effectiveness. J Am Diet Assoc 1985;85:40–5.
  40. Olds DL, Henderson CR Jr, Tatelbaum R, Chamberlin R. Improving the delivery of prenatal care and outcomes of pregnancy: a randomized trial of nurse home visitation. Pediatrics 1986;77:16–28.
  41. Bruce L, Tchabo JG. Nutrition intervention program in a prenatal clinic. Obstet Gynecol 1989;74:310–2.
  42. Morris DL, Berenson AB, Lawson J, Wiemann CM. Comparison of adolescent pregnancy outcomes by prenatal care source. J Reprod Med 1993;38:375–80.
  43. Brown JE, Tharp TM, McKay C, et al. Development of a prenatal weight gain intervention program using social marketing methods. J Nutr Educ 1992;24:21–8.
  44. Brown JE, Tharp T, Finnegan J. Letter to the editor. J Nutr Educ 1996;28:6.
  45. Taffel SM, Keppel KG, Jones GK. Medical advice on maternal weight gain and actual weight gain: results from the 1988 National Maternal and Infant Health Survey. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1993;678:293–305.
  46. Alexander GR, Korenbrot CC. The role of prenatal care in preventing low birth weight. Future Child 1995;5:103–20.
  47. Freda MC, Andersen HF, Damus K, Merkatz IR. Are there differences in information given to private and public prenatal patients? Am J Obstet Gynecol 1993;169:155–60.
  48. Armstrong JE, Weijohn TT. Dietary quality and concerns about body weight of low-income pregnant women. J Am Diet Assoc 1991;91:1280–2.
  49. Davies K, Wardle J. Body image and dieting in pregnancy. J Psychosom Res 1994;38:787–99.
  50. Fairburn CG, Welch SL. The impact of pregnancy on eating habits and attitudes to shape and weight. Int J Eat Disord 1990;9:153–60.
  51. Palmer JL, Jennings GE, Massey L. Development of an assessment form: attitude toward weight gain during pregnancy. J Am Diet Assoc 1985;85:946–9.
  52. Stevens-Simon C, Nakashima II, Andrews D. Weight gain attitudes among pregnant adolescents. J Adolesc Health 1993;14:369–72.
  53. Copper RL, DuBard MB, Goldenberg RL, Oweis AI. The relationship of maternal attitude toward weight gain to weight gain during pregnancy and low birth weight. Obstet Gynecol 1995;85:590–5.
  54. McLean DE, Hatfield-Timajchy K, Wingo PA, Floyd RL. Psychosocial measurement: implications for the study of preterm delivery in black women. In: Rowley D, Tosteson H, eds. Racial differences in preterm delivery: developing a new research paradigm. Am J Prev Med 1993;9:39–81.
  55. Pelto GH. Cultural issues in maternal and child health and nutrition. Soc Sci Med 1987;25:553–9.
  56. Rosenberg EM. Demographic effects of sex-differential nutrition. In: Jerome NW, Kandel RF, Pelto GH, eds. Nutritional anthropology: contemporary approaches to diet and culture. Pleasantville, NY: Redgrave Publishing Company, 1980:181–204.
  57. Hickey CA, Uauy R, Rodriquez LM, Jennings LW. Biosocial predictors of maternal weight near term among low-income black and Hispanic women. Nutr Res 1992;12:337–52.
  58. Hickey CA, Cliver SP, Goldenberg RL, McNeal SF, Hoffman HJ. Relationship of psychosocial status to low prenatal weight gain among nonobese black and white women delivering at term. Obstet Gynecol 1995;86:177–83.
  59. Casanueva E, Legarreta D, Diaz-Barriga M, et al. Weight gain during pregnancy in adolescents: evaluation of a non-nutritional intervention. Rev Invest Clin 1994;46:157–61.
  60. Zuckerman B, Amaro H, Bauchner H, Cabral H. Depressive symptoms during pregnancy: relationship to poor health behaviors. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1989;160:1107–11.
  61. Stevens-Simon C, McAnarney ER. Determinants of weight gain in pregnant adolescents. J Am Diet Assoc 1992;92:1348–51.
  62. Picone TA, Allen LH, Schramm MM, Olsen PN. Pregnancy outcome in North American women. I. Effects of diet, cigarette smoking, and psychological stress on maternal weight gain. Am J Clin Nutr 1982;36:1205–13.
  63. Orr ST, James SA, Miller CA, et al. Psychosocial stressors and low birthweight in an urban population. Am J Prev Med 1996;12:459–66.
  64. Jacobson D. The cultural context of social support and support networks. Med Anthropol Q 1987;1:42–67.
  65. Rowley D, Tosteson H, eds. Racial differences in preterm delivery: developing a new research paradigm. Am J Prev Med 1993; 9(suppl):1–123.
  66. Orr ST, James SA, Casper R. Psychosocial stressors and low birthweight: development of a questionnaire. J Dev Behav Pediatr 1992;13:343–7.
  67. Goldenberg RL, Hickey CA, Cliver SP, Gotlieb S, Woolley TW, Hoffman HJ. Abbreviated scale for the assessment of psychosocial status in pregnancy: development and evaluation. Acta Obstet Gynecol Scand Suppl 1997;76:19–29.
  68. Norbeck JS, Anderson NJ. Psychosocial predictors of pregnancy outcome in low-income black, Hispanic and white women. Nurs Res 1989;38:204–9.
  69. Bragonier JR, Cushner IM, Hobel CJ. Social and personal factors in the etiology of preterm birth. In: Fuchs F, Stubblefield PG, eds. Preterm birth: causes, prevention and management. New York: McMillan, 1984:64–85.
  70. Hobel CJ, Dunkel-Schetter C, Roesch S. Maternal stress as a signal to the fetus. Prenat Neonat Med 1998;3:116–20.
  71. Picone TA, Allen LH, Olsen PN, Ferris ME. Pregnancy outcome in North American women. II. Effects of diet, cigarette smoking, stress, and weight gain on placentas, and on neonatal physical and behavioral characteristics. Am J Clin Nutr 1982;36:1214–24.
  72. Holmes TH, Rahe RH. The social readjustment rating scale. J Psychosom Res 1967;11:213–8.
  73. Caan B, Petitti DB. Food intake and weight gain in pregnant women. Am J Clin Nutr 1983;38:815–7 (letter).
  74. Pagel MD, Smilkstein G, Regen H, Montano D. Psychosocial influences on newborn outcomes. A controlled prospective study. Soc Sci Med 1990;30:597–604.

作者: Carol A Hickey
医学百科App—中西医基础知识学习工具
  • 相关内容
  • 近期更新
  • 热文榜
  • 医学百科App—健康测试工具