Literature
首页医源资料库在线期刊美国临床营养学杂志2000年72卷第5期

Improved iodine status is associated with improved mental performance of schoolchildren in Benin

来源:《美国临床营养学杂志》
摘要:ABSTRACTBackground:Anadequateiodinesupplyinuteroandshortlyafterbirthisknowntobecrucialtoanindividual‘sphysicalandmentaldevelopment。Thequestionofwhetheriodinesupplementationlaterinlifecanexertafavorableinfluenceonthementalperformanceofiodine-deficientpo......

点击显示 收起

Tina van den Briel, Clive E West, Nico Bleichrodt, Fons JR van de Vijver, Eric A Ategbo and Joseph GAJ Hautvast

1 From the Division of Human Nutrition and Epidemiology, Wageningen University, Wageningen, Netherlands; the Department of Work and Organizational Psychology, Free University, Amsterdam; the Department of Social Sciences, Tilburg University, Tilburg, Netherlands; and the Department of Food and Nutrition, the Faculty of Agriculture, National University of Benin, Cotonou, Benin.

2 Supported in part by a grant from the Nestlé Foundation for the Study of the Problems of Nutrition in the World and by Wageningen University.

3 Address reprint requests to CE West, Division of Human Nutrition and Epidemiology, Wageningen University, PO Box 8129, 6700 EV Wageningen, Netherlands. E-mail: clive.west{at}staff.nutepi.wau.nl.


ABSTRACT  
Background: An adequate iodine supply in utero and shortly after birth is known to be crucial to an individual's physical and mental development. The question of whether iodine supplementation later in life can exert a favorable influence on the mental performance of iodine-deficient populations was addressed in various studies, but with contradictory results.

Objective: The aim of this study was to examine the effect of an improvement in iodine status on mental and psychomotor performance of schoolchildren (7–11 y) who were moderately to severely iodine deficient.

Design: The study, which was originally planned as a double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled intervention, was carried out in an iodine-deficient population of schoolchildren (n = 196) in northern Benin. As the population began to have access to iodized salt during the 1-y intervention period, the study population was split post hoc—on the basis of urinary iodine concentrations—into a group with improved iodine status and a group with unchanged iodine status. Changes in mental and psychomotor performance over the intervention period were compared.

Results: Children with increased urinary iodine concentrations had a significantly greater increase in performance on the combination of mental tests than did the group with no change in urinary iodine concentrations.

Conclusions: An improvement in iodine status, rather than iodine status itself, determined mental performance in this population, which was initially iodine deficient. These findings suggest a "catch-up" effect in terms of mental performance.

Key Words: Iodine deficiency • mental performance • school-children • urinary iodine concentration • Benin • West Africa


INTRODUCTION  
The public health importance of an adequate iodine supply for the physical and mental well-being of humankind has been well described (1, 2). Most studies on the relation between iodine status and mental performance of children have concentrated on effects of iodine supply in utero and shortly after birth on mental and psychomotor development (3–7). The most commonly held premise is that the mental capacity of children, once affected by iodine deficiency in early life, is impaired permanently. Few intervention studies have been carried out to examine whether mental and psychomotor performance of growing children, while iodine deficient, may still benefit from iodine supplementation. In a double-blind, placebo-controlled study that examined the effect of iodine supplementation on the mental performance of children aged 5.5–12 y, Bautista et al (8) found no difference between supplemented and nonsupplemented children. In a double-blind, placebo-controlled intervention study in Malawi, iodine supplementation significantly improved the performance of 6–8-y-old children on many mental tests (9).

The purpose of this study was to determine whether the findings in Malawi could be confirmed in a different setting and, if so, to examine which specific aspects of cognitive functioning are influenced most by supplementation. The research was planned as a double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled study involving oral doses of iodized oil in schoolchildren in an iodine-deficient area in northern Benin, West Africa. However, much earlier than expected—halfway through the intervention period—iodized salt was introduced into the study area. As iodine became available to both groups, the hypothesis that iodine supplementation improves mental performance could no longer be tested. Therefore, we tested whether children whose iodine status improved over the intervention period showed a greater improvement in mental and psychomotor performance than did children whose iodine status changed to a limited extent or not at all.


SUBJECTS AND METHODS  
Study area and subjects
The study was carried out in the district of Basila, province of Atacora, in northern Benin, where prevalence rates of goiter in schoolchildren aged 6–12 y varied from 20% to 60% (A Doh, EA Ategbo, unpublished observations, 1994). The population, mostly Anii, was engaged in subsistence farming, with cotton as the sole source of cash income. Food security was a seasonal concern. Polygamy was common and households were made up of extended families. The 4 study villages had neither electricity nor clean drinking water.

Children aged 7–11 y from standards (grades) 2 and 3 in the 4 primary schools in the study area were considered for enrollment. In 2 of these 4 grades, all female children had been given an iodized oil supplement in the previous year; therefore, only boys were selected from these 2 schools.

The study was approved by the health and education authorities of the province of Atacora and by the Medical Ethics Committee, Division of Human Nutrition and Epidemiology, Wageningen University. The aim of the study was explained to local administrative and traditional authorities, parents, and teachers. After verbal approval was obtained from local authorities, the parents of the children, and the parent-teacher association, all children selected were examined physically by a clinician. Several children with skin or respiratory infections and malaria were treated. No children were excluded on health grounds.

Study design
Children were stratified by school, school class, and sex and subsequently were matched on the basis of similar age and height-for-age. From each pair of children, one child was randomly allocated to 1 of 2 groups. The groups were then randomly allocated to receive iodine supplements or a placebo. The study was double blind, randomized, and placebo controlled with the codes being broken only after completion of the final test. Iodized oil (Lipiodol UF 7; 540 g I/L) and the placebo (poppy-seed oil) were provided by Guerbet Laboratories (Aulnay-sous-Bois, France). Iodized oil and poppy-seed oil were dispensed as a single dose (1.0 mL) administered orally with a Swift 7 dispenser (English Glass Company, Leicester, United Kingdom) in January 1996.

Baseline anthropometric measurements were made and urine and blood samples were collected in October and November 1995. Baseline mental development tests were performed during the same period. All measurements and tests were repeated in October and November 1996. Additional urine samples were collected 1 wk and 5 mo after supplementation.

Somatic and biochemical indicators
Anthropometric measurements were made in duplicate. Height was measured to the nearest millimeter with a microtoise (Stanley Tools, Besançon, France). Weight was measured to the nearest 0.25 kg with a spring scale. Venous blood was drawn from the antecubital vein; one drop of whole blood was then immediately applied to a filter paper card (grade 903; Schleicher and Schuell, Keene, NH). These cards were air-dried for 1–2 h and packed in polyethylene bags before being frozen. Hemoglobin was assessed by using a Hemocue (Helsingborg, Sweden). Serum samples were prepared and frozen before being transported. Samples of urine (25 mL), to which some crystals of thymol were added, were collected. Blood-spot cards and frozen samples of urine and serum were transported to the Micronutrient Research Laboratory, University of Ghana at Legon, Accra, for analysis of urinary iodine [chloric acid digestion followed by the Sandell-Kolthoff reaction (10)], thyroid-stimulating hormone (TSH) in blood spots (SpectraScreen Dried Blood TSH EIA Kit; IEM Diagnostica, Reflex Industries, Santee, CA), and serum ferritin (enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay) within the next 6–9 mo. Frozen serum samples were also transported to the Laboratory for Endocrinology, Amsterdam Medical Centre, Netherlands, for assessment of thyroglobulin (radioimmunoassay), free thyroxine (T4) with time-resolved fluoroimmunoassay (Delfia; Wallac Oy, Turku, Finland), and TSH with an immunoluminometric assay (Brahms Diagnostica GmbH, Berlin).

Mental and psychomotor development tests
Because no comprehensive battery of mental tests has been developed for use in French-speaking West Africa, many tests consisting of nonverbal and abstract pictorial material from the French Kaufman ABC test battery (11) were pretested in a nearby village. Apart from the sequential memory test (hand movements), most tests contained images unknown to the children. Subsequently, a battery of tests, mostly nonverbal, was composed, which have been used under conditions comparable with those found in rural Benin (ie, tests requiring little or no vocabulary skills of the child being tested, thus avoiding confounding by education-related language skills). These tests covered, as much as possible, aspects of fluid intelligence, as opposed to crystallized intelligence (12–14). Fluid intelligence is regarded as one of the major constituents of intelligence; it refers to the ability to reason by analogy, to apprehend an unfamiliar configuration, and to construct or extract a solution. Crystallized intelligence refers to subject-matter proficiency acquired in the past, which is reflected in results of tests measuring such aspects as vocabulary, arithmetic, or factual knowledge. The mental test battery included the following 8 tests: block design (15), 5 tests (closure, concentration, exclusion, fluency, and mazes) from the African Child Intelligence Test (16), hand movements (11), and colored progressive matrixes (17). These tests, including the mental ability tested in each according to factors reported by Thurstone (18) and Ekstrom et al (19), are described in Table 1. In addition, 2 psychomotor tests—pegboard and ball throwing—were carried out. A psychologist (NB) trained 2 university graduates and 2 teachers from Benin to conduct the tests, which were subsequently pretested among schoolchildren from the study area who were not included in the study population. The testers worked in pairs. All 4 testers started with the same 3 tests in a fixed sequence. The remaining tests were administered by either one tester or the other, with the same tester always being responsible for the same set of tests. All children were given a snack before testing, which took place between 0900 and 1200 in a room that was quiet and free of distractions. The 8 mental tests plus the pegboard test took 50–60 min, after which the children were taken outside for the ball-throwing test. Because of the diversity and short duration of the tests, children did not become tired or bored. A simple reaction-time test and a choice reaction-time test, both measuring information processing time, as well as a tapping test measuring manual dexterity and accuracy were administered at the end of the intervention period but not on the same day as the mental tests.


View this table:
TABLE 1. Mental development tests used in the study  
Data analysis
Data analysis was related to the modified study design. Children were allocated to new groups on the basis of the magnitude of change in urinary iodine concentration over the intervention period. Differences in changes in mental performance between these groups were assessed by using Student's t test. If not normally distributed, log transformations were made. Factor analysis, based on the intercorrelation of the tests, was carried out by using a principal components analysis with VARIMAX, an orthogonal factor rotation method. This enabled us to examine the construct validity of the test battery. All data were processed and analyzed by using SPSS-PC software (version 6.1 for WINDOWS; SPSS Inc, Chicago). Anthropometric indexes were calculated by using EPI-INFO (version 6.02; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, Atlanta).


RESULTS  
Initially, 211 children were enrolled in the study; 13 children left school or moved out of the area by the end of the intervention period and 2 children could not be located during urine collection. The socioeconomic status of the families included in the studies was generally poor. Families were large, landholding size was relatively small, and education levels among adults were low (Table 2).


View this table:
TABLE 2. General characteristics of subjects and their families  
Nutritional status
Thirty-three percent of the children were stunted [height-for-age < -2 SD of the National Center for Health Statistics (NCHS) reference; 20], 17% had low weight-for-age (< -2 SD of the NCHS reference), and 2% were wasted (weight-for-height < -2 SD of the NCHS reference) (Table 2). The proportion of children with anemia (hemoglobin <110 g/L) was 33%, whereas 11% had moderately to severely depleted iron stores (serum ferritin concentration <18 µg/L). With an initial median urinary iodine concentration of 0.16 µmol/L (20.6 µg/L), the study population was classified as moderately to severely iodine deficient. At the end of the intervention period, both the original placebo and the iodine-supplemented groups showed clearly improved iodine status. However, the total study population was still classified as mildly iodine deficient on the basis of median urinary iodine concentrations. The initial urinary iodine concentration was positively correlated (Spearman) with the serum concentration of free T4 (r = 0.19, P = 0.007) and negatively correlated with serum concentrations of thyroglobulin (r = -0.51, P = 0.000) and TSH (r = -0.34, P = 0.000). At the end of the study, the urinary iodine concentration was related to the serum thyroglobulin concentration only (r = -0.21, P = 0.003).

Mental performance
Correlations between the scores on the different tests at baseline were mostly positive (Table 3), in line with what is usually found (21). Factor analysis of the series of tests carried out by using VARIMAX produced 3 factors: block design, closure, concentration, exclusion, fluency, and maze tests loaded on one factor; the hand movements test loaded on the second factor; and the colored progressive matrixes, or Raven test, loaded on the third factor (Table 4). This pattern is similar to the pattern found in a study that used an intelligence test for Dutch, Spanish, and Indian children, and to that found when this battery of tests was compared with the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (22–24). The first factor refers to spatial and perceptual reasoning skills, the second factor to sequential memory, and the third factor to general intelligence, often referred to as g (21, 25). The overall changes in performance over the intervention period were small but positive. The test-retest correlation of the full test battery in the unchanged group was 0.83 (P = 0.000).


View this table:
TABLE 3. Correlation between mental development tests in the total group at the beginning of the study1  

View this table:
TABLE 4. VARIMAX-rotated factor matrix of results on mental tests at the beginning of the intervention  
Mental performance in relation to iodine status
Children were categorized both at the beginning and at the end of the study with respect to their urinary iodine concentrations (Table 5): normal-mild (>0.40 µmol I/L urine), moderate (0.16–0.40 µmol I/L urine), or severe (<0.16 µmol I/L urine) based on criteria for establishing the severity of iodine deficiency as a public health problem (26). The categorization at the end of the study was based on mean urinary iodine excretion 5 and 11 mo after supplementation. Subsequently, children were allocated to 1 of 2 groups, the criterion for allocation being whether or not their iodine status had improved. Urinary iodine concentrations increased considerably in about two-thirds of the children (ie, status changed from severe to moderate, from severe to normal-mild, or from moderate to normal-mild)—the "improved" group. The second, or "unchanged," group consisted of children whose urinary iodine concentrations remained unchanged [ie, status remained moderate or normal-mild or, in a few cases (n = 7), from normal-mild to moderate]. None of the children in this group were severely deficient at the beginning or at the end of the study. Note that the unchanged group had, on average, better initial and end-of-study iodine status than did the improved group (Table 5). Both groups consisted of supplemented and nonsupplemented children, but proportions were not significantly different (chi-square test). Although the improved group was older than the unchanged group by 7 mo, both groups had comparable blood hemoglobin concentrations, anthropometric and socioeconomic indexes, and initial scores on the mental tests. No correlation was observed between age and change in mental performance in either of the intervention groups or in the study population as a whole (data not shown).


View this table:
TABLE 5. Indicators of iodine status at the beginning and end of the intervention in groups categorized as improved or unchanged on the basis of their urinary iodine concentrations1  
The performance at baseline and at the end of the study, as well as the changes in performance on the series of mental tests during the study period, were expressed as z scores. Thus, the z scores of the improved and unchanged groups together were zero, both at baseline and at the end of the study. The mean initial z score of the unchanged group (n = 68) was -0.02 ± 0.58, whereas that of the improved group (n = 128) was 0.01 ± 0.58. Mean z scores at the end of the study were -0.10 ± 0.59 in the unchanged group and 0.05 ± 0.57 in the improved group. The z scores for the change in performance (ie, the differences between scores on each test at baseline and at the end of the study period) in the unchanged group were set at 0 with an SD at 1. Thus, the scores of the improved group represent the difference from the unchanged group (Table 6). Comparison of the performance on the range of tests shows a consistent pattern in favor of the improved group and the overall results in the improved group were significantly better than those in the unchanged group. With respect to the reaction-time and tapping tests, no effect of change in iodine status was shown.


View this table:
TABLE 6. Change in mental performance during intervention in the group of children with improved urinary iodine concentrations1  

DISCUSSION  
This study showed that improvements in urinary iodine status in the study population reflected significantly improved mental performance on a combination of tests. Children whose urinary iodine concentrations were basically unchanged during the intervention period showed less progress in performance, even though their iodine statuses as measured by several variables were, on average, better than those of their improved counterparts both initially and at the end of the study. These findings indicate "catch-up," ie, improvement toward full potential as a result of iodine supplementation.

The improvements in test performance found in the present study were most pronounced on the exclusion test and the colored progressive matrixes, suggesting an improvement in general abstract reasoning. Improvement was also seen on the test for verbal fluency, in agreement with the results of a study in Malawi in which improved verbal fluency was one of the most pronounced effects of iodine supplementation (9). Because all the tests used, except the colored progressive matrixes, have a time limit within which a response must be given, these findings may indicate improvement in the level of task performance, improvement in speed of task performance, or both.

Improved attention or concentration may facilitate improvement in mental function (21). According to Tiwari et al (27), iodine-deficient children are slow learners with a concurrent low "motivation to achieve." These authors ascribe poor performance to neurologic impairment and to a paucity of psychological stimulation. In hypothyroidism, mood disorders—including depression, social withdrawal, and paucity of speech—are common phenomena (28). Thus, it may be that poor performance under conditions such as those of our study reflects a general state of apathy, accounting not only for a lack of motivation but also for other factors important in cognitive functioning, such as attentiveness and concentration.

The marked improvement in mental and psychomotor performance as a result of iodine supplementation, which was seen in a study of schoolchildren in Malawi [>10 intelligence quotient (IQ) points (9)], was not achieved in the present study. Although the iodine status of most of the children in the present study had improved substantially by the end of the intervention period, the improvement in mental performance was only 5 IQ points. The Beninese children were older than the children in Malawi (mean age: 7.1 y), but otherwise comparable in terms of initial iodine and iron statuses and in anthropometric indexes. The improvement in iodine status, whether through iodized oil supplementation or through consumption of iodized salt, may have come too late in the life of the Beninese children to enable comparable catch-up to take place.

The functional classification of children with respect to degrees of iodine deficiency remains problematic for several reasons. First, there is as yet no universally accepted single indicator for iodine status in this age group and, second, cutoff points enabling different degrees of iodine deficiency to be distinguished are based on populations rather than on individuals. The 4 iodine-status variables that we measured could not be captured in 1 variable by factor analysis, which was explained by the fact that each of these variables reflects different facets of iodine metabolism. Although initial urinary iodine concentrations indicated serious iodine deficiency in our study population, initial serum TSH and free T4 concentrations in our group were found to be within the normal range (Table 5). These findings concur with those of Benmiloud et al (29), Pardede et al (30), and Untoro (31), who also found values for TSH and free T4 in the normal range in studies of iodine-deficient populations. These authors, therefore, maintain that urinary iodine excretion is the best outcome indicator for interventions involving iodine supplementation. Although TSH and free T4 concentrations improved significantly during our study, it might be argued that the normal range of TSH and free T4 values is too wide, at least for this age group. Although the thyroglobulin concentration is considered to be very sensitive to changes in iodine metabolism, assay methods for thyroglobulin are not standardized among laboratories and, therefore, normal ranges and cutoff points for various degrees of iodine status cannot be established. For these reasons, our subjects were categorized on the basis of their urinary iodine concentrations. Although regarded as the best indicator of iodine status at the population level, the urinary iodine concentration also has its limitations, especially when used at the individual level. This is because it primarily reflects the previous day's iodine intake, which may not represent long-term intake. In addition, urinary iodine concentrations vary throughout the day. Thus, caution should be exercised in the interpretation of results.

Because of the current considerable rate of progress in universal salt iodization, further research in this field is increasingly difficult to carry out. However, constraints in iodine supply and metabolism, both in individuals and in population groups with insufficient access to sources of iodine, will continue to call for better insight into the relations between iodine status and mental functioning. Although our study showed that restoration of impaired psychoneurologic processes is to some extent still possible in schoolchildren, many questions remain. Is there an age threshold beyond which restoration is no longer possible? Are different aspects of cognitive functioning restored at different speeds? Which iodine variables are most closely associated with changes in cognitive functioning? This study was not designed to answer these questions. However, it does indicate that mental performance of iodine-deficient children is positively influenced by iodine supplementation, whether through the use of iodized salt or the administration of iodized oil.


REFERENCES  

  1. Bleichrodt N, Born MP. A metaanalysis of research on iodine and its relationship to cognitive development. In: Stanbury JB, ed. The damaged brain of iodine deficiency. New York: Cognizant Communication Corporation, 1994.
  2. Hetzel BS, Pandav CS, eds. SOS for a billion—the conquest of iodine deficiency disorders. New York: Oxford University Press, 1994.
  3. Pharoah POD, Connolly KJ. Effects of maternal iodine supplementation during pregnancy. Arch Dis Child 1991;66:145–7.
  4. Fierro-Benitez R, Cazar R, Stanbury JB, Rodriguez F, Fierro-Renoy F. Effects on schoolchildren of prophylaxis of mothers with iodized oil in an area of iodine deficiency. J Endocrinol Invest 1988;11:327–35.
  5. Pharoah POD, Connolly KJ, Ekins R, Harding A. Maternal thyroid hormone levels in pregnancy and the subsequent cognitive and motor performance of the children. Clin Endocrinol 1984;21:265–70.
  6. Cao XY, Jiang XM, Dou ZH, et al. Timing of vulnerability of the brain to iodine deficiency in endemic cretinism. N Engl J Med 1994;331:1739–44.
  7. Delong GR. Iodine and brain development. Dev Med Child Neurol 1996;38:279–80.
  8. Bautista A, Barker PA, Dunn JT, Sanchez M, Kaiser DL. The effects of oral iodized oil on intelligence, thyroid status, and somatic growth in school-age children from an area of endemic goiter. Am J Clin Nutr 1982;35:127–34.
  9. Shrestha R. Effect of iodine and iron supplementation on physical, psychomotor and mental development in primary school children in Malawi. PhD thesis. Wageningen Agricultural University, Wageningen, Netherlands, 1994.
  10. Dunn JT, Crutchfield HE, Gutekunst R, Dunn AD. Methods of measuring iodine in urine. Wageningen, Netherlands: ICCIDD/UNICEF/ WHO, 1993.
  11. Kaufman A, Kaufman N. K-ABC; batterie pour l'examen psychologique de l'enfant. (K-ABC; battery for the psychological testing of the child.) Paris: Les Editions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée, 1993 (in French).
  12. Cattell RB. Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: a critical experiment. J Educ Psychol 1963;54:1–22.
  13. Cattell RB. La théorie d'intelligence fluide et cristallisé, sa relation avec les tests ‘culture fair' et sa vérifications chez les enfants de 9 a 12 ans. (The theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence, its relation with ‘culture fair' tests and its verification with children of 9 to 12 years.) Rev Psychol Appl 1967;17:134–54 (in French).
  14. Cattell RB. The theory of fluid and crystallized general intelligence checked at the 5–6 year-old level. Br J Educ Psychol 1967;37:209–44.
  15. Wechsler D. Wechsler preschool and primary scale of intelligence, WPPSI. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation, 1967.
  16. Drenth PJD, van der Vlier H, Muinde NP, Otaala B, Omari IM, Opolot JA. Jaribio akili mtoto Afrika. (African child intelligence test.) Amsterdam: Free University of Amsterdam, 1980 (in Kiswahili).
  17. Raven JC. Coloured progressive matrices. 10th ed. Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford Psychologists Press Ltd, 1993.
  18. Thurstone LL. Primary mental abilities. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 1969.
  19. Ekstrom RB, French JW, Harman HH. Cognitive factors: their identification and replication. Multivariate Behav Res Monogr 1979;2:1–74.
  20. World Health Organization. Measuring change in nutritional status—guidelines for assessing the nutritional impact of supplementary feeding programmes for vulnerable groups. Geneva: WHO, 1983.
  21. Carroll JB. Human cognitive abilities; a survey of factor-analytic studies. 1st ed. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press, 1993.
  22. Bleichrodt N, Resing W, Drenth PJD, Zaal J. Intelligentie-meting bij kinderen (Measuring intelligence in children.) Lisse, Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger BV, 1987 (in Dutch).
  23. Bleichrodt N, Escobar del Rey F, Morreale de Escobar G, Garcia I, Rubio C. Iodine deficiency, implications for mental and psychomotor development in children. In: Delong GR, Robbins J, Condliffe PG, eds. Iodine and the brain. New York: Plenum Publishing Corporation, 1989:269–88.
  24. Bleichrodt N, Hoksbergen RAC, Khire U. Cross-cultural testing of intelligence. Cross-Cult Res 1999;33:3–25.
  25. Cronbach LJ. Essentials of psychological testing. 5th ed. New York: HarperCollins, 1990.
  26. WHO/UNICEF/ICCIDD. Indicators for assessing iodine deficiency disorders and their control through salt iodization. Geneva: WHO, 1994. (WHO/NUT/94.6.)
  27. Tiwari BD, Godbole MM, Chattopadhyay N, Mandal A, Mithal A. Learning disabilities and poor motivation to achieve due to prolonged iodine deficiency. Am J Clin Nutr 1996;63:782–6.
  28. Esposito S, Prange AJJ, Golden RN. The thyroid axis and mood disorders: overview and future prospects. Psychopharmacol Bull 1997; 33:205–17.
  29. Benmiloud M, Chaouki ML, Gutekunst R, Teichert HM, Wood WG, Dunn JT. Oral iodized oil for correcting iodine deficiency: optimal dosing and outcome indicator selection. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1994;79:20–4.
  30. Pardede LV, Hardjowasito W, Gross R, et al. Urinary iodine excretion is the most appropriate outcome indicator for iodine deficiency at field conditions at district level. J Nutr 1998;128:1122–6.
  31. Untoro J. Use of oral iodized oil to control iodine deficiency in Indonesia. PhD thesis. Wageningen, Netherlands: Wageningen Agricultural University, 1999.
Received for publication July 22, 1999. Accepted for publication March 29, 2000.


作者: Tina van den Briel
医学百科App—中西医基础知识学习工具
  • 相关内容
  • 近期更新
  • 热文榜
  • 医学百科App—健康测试工具