Literature
首页医源资料库在线期刊美国临床营养学杂志2000年72卷第6期

Energy requirements derived from total energy expenditure and energy deposition during the first 2 y of life

来源:《美国临床营养学杂志》
摘要:ABSTRACTBackground:Currentrecommendationsforenergyintakeofchildrenarederivedfromobservedintakes。Derivingenergyrequirementsonthebasisofenergyexpenditureanddepositionisscientificallymorerationalthanisusingtheobservationalapproachandisnowpossiblewithdataonto......

点击显示 收起

Nancy F Butte, William W Wong, Judy M Hopkinson, Carolyn J Heinz, Nitesh R Mehta and E O'Brian Smith

1 From the US Department of Agriculture/Agricultural Research Service, Children's Nutrition Research Center, Department of Pediatrics, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston.

2 The contents of this article do not necessarily reflect the views or policies of the US Department of Agriculture, and mention of trade names, commercial products, or organizations does not imply endorsement by the US government.

3 Supported in part by the US Department of Agriculture/Agricultural Research Service under Cooperative Agreement 58-6250-6001. Formulas were donated by Ross Laboratories.

4 Address reprint requests to NF Butte, Children's Nutrition Research Center, 1100 Bates, Houston, TX 77030. E-mail: nbutte{at}bcm.tmc.edu.


ABSTRACT  
Background: Current recommendations for energy intake of children are derived from observed intakes. Deriving energy requirements on the basis of energy expenditure and deposition is scientifically more rational than is using the observational approach and is now possible with data on total energy expenditure (TEE), growth, and body composition.

Objectives: The objectives of this study were 1) to define energy requirements during the first 2 y of life on the basis of TEE and energy deposition; 2) to test effects of sex, age, and feeding mode on energy requirements; and 3) to determine physical activity.

Design: TEE, sleeping metabolic rate, anthropometry, and body composition were measured in 76 infants. TEE was measured with doubly labeled water, sleeping metabolic rate with respiratory calorimetry, and body composition with a multicomponent model.

Results: Total energy requirements were 2.23, 2.59, 2.97, 3.38, 3.72, and 4.15 MJ/d at 3, 6, 9, 12, 18, and 24 mo, respectively. Energy deposition (in MJ/d) decreased significantly over time (P = 0.001) and was lower in breast-fed than in formula-fed infants (P = 0.01). Energy requirements were 80% of current recommendations. Energy requirements differed by age (P = 0.001), feeding group (P = 0.03), and sex (P = 0.03). Adjusted for weight or fat-free mass and fat mass, energy requirements still differed by feeding group but not by age or sex. Temperament and motor development did not affect TEE.

Conclusion: The TEE and energy-deposition data of these healthy, thriving children provide strong evidence that current recommendations for energy intake in the first 2 y of life should be revised.

Key Words: Energy requirements • energy expenditure • energy deposition • doubly labeled water • sleeping metabolic rate • physical activity level • infants • toddlers


INTRODUCTION  
The energy requirements of infants and young children are the energy intakes that will balance energy expenditure (EE) at a physical activity level (PAL) consistent with normal development and allow for deposition of tissues at a rate consistent with health. In older children and adults, energy requirements are based on basal metabolic rates and an allowance for PAL. Because it was not possible to specify with any confidence the allowance for a desirable PAL in infants and young children, the 1985 FAO/WHO/UNU recommendations for the energy intake from birth to 10 y of age were derived from the observed intakes of healthy, thriving children (1). Energy requirements of infants were based on energy intakes compiled by Whitehead et al (2); 5% was added to compensate for underestimation of intake. Energy requirements of toddlers were estimated from intake data compiled by Ferro-Luzzi and Durnin (3). Implicit in this approach is the assumption that ad libitum intakes reflect desirable intakes. However, energy intakes are not inherently constant but are influenced by external factors. For instance, downward secular trends in the energy intakes of infants have been attributed to changes in breast-feeding rates, the formulation of infant formula, and the timing of food supplementation (2). Energy requirements of young children derived from EE and energy-deposition values would be scientifically more rational than the use of observed intakes. Implicit in the approach based on EE and energy deposition is knowledge of what constitutes developmentally appropriate PALs and normal growth and body composition. Although the energy requirement for growth relative to maintenance is small, except for during the first months of life, satisfactory growth is a sensitive indicator of whether energy needs are being met. To determine the energy cost of growth, the energy content of the newly synthesized tissues must be estimated, preferably from the separate costs of protein and fat deposition.

With the emergence of information on total EE (TEE) by the doubly labeled water (DLW) method, the energy requirements of young children can be estimated on the basis of EE as first shown by Prentice et al (4). DLW measurements of TEE include basal metabolism, thermogenesis, the synthetic cost of growth, and PAL. Although application of the DLW method in young children is subject to errors, the method was validated in infants (5–8). In 1996, the need to revise energy and protein requirements, and the data necessary to do this, were considered (9, 10). It was concluded that current recommendations for energy intake for children aged <2 y were too high; however, revision would require expansion of the database on TEE of children, especially children aged 6–24 mo. An unresolved issue pertinent to the revision of energy recommendations is whether differences in energy utilization observed between breast-fed (BF) and formula-fed (FF) infants in early infancy persist into the second year of life (11).

In this study, energy requirements during the first 2 y of life were derived from the sum of TEE and energy deposition. Factors potentially affecting energy requirements, including sex, age, body size and composition, feeding group, temperament, and motor development, were tested. In addition, PALs consistent with normal development were determined from measurements of TEE and sleeping metabolic rate (SMR).


SUBJECTS AND METHODS  
Study design and subjects
Repeated measurements of TEE, SMR, body composition, temperament, and motor development were performed in 76 healthy term infants at the Children's Nutrition Research Center (CNRC) at ages 3, 6, 9, 12, 18, and 24 mo. Body composition was also measured at age 0.5 mo. Feeding practices and morbidity were ascertained at each visit. Seventy-two children completed the 24-mo study. By study design, the infants were either exclusively BF (n = 40) or FF (n = 36) from birth to 4 mo of age; thereafter, the feeding group was at the discretion of the infants' parents. This study was approved by the Baylor Affiliates Review Board for Human Subject Research and informed, written consent was obtained from each child's mother.

Infants were born healthy at full term to women with unremarkable health histories and pregnancies. The mean maternal age (±SD) was 28.8 ± 4.2 y. Median gravidity and parity were 2 (range: 1–5) and 0 (range: 0–3), respectively. Maternal height and prepregnancy weight averaged 164.3 ± 6.0 cm and 61.1 ± 8.7 kg, respectively. Gestational weight gain was 16.2 ± 5.3 kg. Family income was distributed as follows: <$20000, 8%; between $20000 and $34999, 24%; between $35000 and $49999, 17%; and >$50000, 51%. The above characteristics did not differ by infant feeding group; attained level of education, however, was higher in the BF (16.6 ± 2.6 y) than in the FF (14.4 ± 1.9 y) group (P = 0.001).

The infants were admitted to the CNRC Metabolic Research Unit from 1000 to 1700 for the series of measurements. Anthropometric measurements were performed 1 h after the infants were fed. 2H218O was administered orally with a syringe 30 min after the subjects were fed to avoid regurgitation of the dose. The 15-min whole-body 40K counting and the dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry (DXA) measurements were usually made in the younger infants while they slept and in the older children while they were entertained with a video. The SMR was measured during an afternoon nap.

Infant feeding practices and morbidity
At each visit, infant feeding practices and morbidity were ascertained. The mothers were asked about breast-feeding and use of infant formula, solid foods, beverages, and vitamin-mineral supplements. The mothers were asked to recall any infant illness in the preceding study interval, including the type, duration, and treatment of the illness.

Motor development and temperament
The Bayley Scales of Infant Motor Development were administered by a trained examiner at each age. With correction for the child's age, the raw score was converted to the Psychomotor Development Index (PDI). The temperament of the children was assessed with use of the Carey Temperament Questionnaires (12). The Early Infancy Temperament Questionnaire (12) was used at 3 mo of age. The Infant Temperament Questionnaire was used at 6 and 9 mo of age. The Toddler Temperament Scale (12) was used at 12 and 24 mo of age. These questionnaires consist of 76–97 items on which the mother is asked to rate the actual current behavior of her child in a variety of situations. These responses are converted into category scores from 0 to 6 for 9 characteristics: activity, biological rhythm, initial approach or withdrawal, adaptability, intensity, mood, persistence or attention span, distractibility, and sensory threshold. These categories are used to group infants into 1 of 5 diagnostic clusters: difficult, intermediate-high (difficult), intermediate-low (easy), easy, and slow to warm up.

Anthropometry
The infants were weighed naked on an electronic integrating scale 30 min after being fed (Sartorius MC1, LC34; Gottingen, Germany; precision: ±1.0 g). Crown-to-heel length was measured on a recumbent infant board to the nearest 1 mm by 2 trained persons (Holtain Limited, Crymych, United Kingdom). The National Center for Health Statistics (NCHS) growth reference was used to evaluate these children (13).

Multicomponent body-composition model
Body composition was estimated from measurements of total body water, total body potassium, and bone mineral content by using a modified version (14) of the multicomponent model published by Fomon et al (15). Total body water was estimated from deuterium dilution space (NH) as part of the DLW method at age 3–24 mo. At age 0.5 mo, NH was calculated from the average of two 3- to 5-h postdose urine samples by the plateau method after an oral dose of 50 mg 2H2O/kg body weight. NH was converted to total body water by dividing by 1.04. Total body potassium was estimated from the 40K naturally present in the child's body by using a whole-body counter (16). For the 15-min count, photons are detected by 12 photon-sensitive NaI(Tl) detectors arranged in 2 arrays above and below the child's body in the low-background whole-body counter. DXA was used to estimate bone mineral content with a Hologic QDR-2000 instrument by using INFANT WHOLE BODY ANALYSIS software (version 5.56-5.71P; Hologic, Inc, Waltham, MA) at ages 0.5, 12, and 24 mo. For the time points at which DXA scans were not performed, bone mineral content was predicted from an equation based on the linear regression of bone mineral content on total body potassium. Energy deposition was computed from the change in protein and fat mass (FM) between adjacent study intervals. The energy equivalents for protein and fat deposition were taken as 23.6 kJ/g protein and 38.7 kJ/g fat, respectively. If body-composition data were missing from one time interval, energy deposition was computed from the change in weight multiplied by the mean energy cost of growth: 20.1, 10.0, 7.9, 10.0, 10.8, and 11.7 kJ/g weight gain at ages 3, 6, 9, 12, 18, and 24 mo, respectively.

Total energy expenditure by the doubly labeled water method
TEE was measured by the DLW method (17). Studies were completed successfully in 351 tests. Parental failure to complete the 10-d urine collection as instructed was the most common cause of unusable data. After collection of a baseline urine sample, the child received by mouth 100 mg 2H2O (Cambridge Isotope Laboratories, Andover, MA) and 125 mg H2 18O (Cambridge Isotope Laboratories) per kg body weight. One daily urine sample was collected at home for the next 10 d by using cotton balls placed within the child's diaper (18). Urine was expressed from soaked cotton balls with a 50-mL syringe into O-ring-sealed sample vials and then frozen at -20°C. The time of collection was recorded.

Urine samples were analyzed for hydrogen and oxygen isotope ratio measurements by gas isotope ratio mass spectrometry (19). For hydrogen isotope ratio measurements, 10 µL urine without further treatment was reduced to hydrogen gas with 200 mg Zn reagent at 500°C for 30 min (20). The ratios of 2H to 1H of the hydrogen gas were measured with a Delta-E gas isotope ratio mass spectrometer (Finnigan MAT, San Jose, CA). For oxygen isotope ratio measurements, 100 µL urine was allowed to equilibrate with 300 mbar CO2 of known 18O content at 25°C for 10 h with use of a VG ISOPREP-18 water–carbon dioxide equilibration system (VG Isogas, Ltd, Cheshire, United Kingdom). At the end of the equilibration, the ratio of 18O to 16O of the carbon dioxide was measured with a VG SIRA-12 gas isotope ratio mass spectrometer (VG Isogas, Ltd).

NH and the 18O dilution space (NO) were calculated as follows:

RESULTS  
Subjects
The mean (±SD) birth weight and length of the 76 infants were 3.42 ± 0.44 kg and 50.56 ± 2.24 cm, respectively, and did not differ significantly between the BF (n = 40) and FF (n = 36) infants. The mean gestational age was 39.1 ± 1.3 wk. The racial distribution by maternal lineage was 55 white, 7 African American, 11 Hispanic, and 3 Asian infants. The average duration of breast-feeding was 11.4 ± 5.8 mo. The percentages of BF children still breast-fed at ages 3, 6, 9, 12, 18, and 24 mo were 100%, 80%, 58%, 38%, 15%, and 5%, respectively. The average duration of formula feeding was 4.4 ± 4.5 mo in the BF group and 11.9 ± 3.8 mo in the FF group. The percentages of BF children given formula at 3, 6, 9, 12, 18, and 24 mo were 0%, 40%, 48%, 30%, 10%, and 2%, respectively. For the FF children, the corresponding percentages were 100%, 100%, 94%, 47%, 6%, and 0%. Seventeen (42%) of the BF infants were not given formula.

Aside from common childhood illnesses, the children were healthy. Maternal recall of the occurrence, type, and duration of infant illness did not differ significantly between the BF and FF infants at any age, with the exception that more illnesses were reported for BF than for FF infants at age 12 mo. The proportion of mothers returning to work, the number of hours worked, and the type of childcare arrangement did not differ significantly between the BF and the FF infants. On average, 62%, 51%, 48%, 51%, 53%, and 37% of the infants stayed at home, whereas others attended daycare centers or private residence facilities at age 3, 6, 9, 12, 18, and 24 mo, respectively.

Anthropometry and body composition
Anthropometric and body-composition measurements are summarized in Table 1. Statistical testing was performed with control for initial values at age 0.5 mo. A 3-way interaction among feeding group, sex, and age was detected for weight. Weight was higher in the FF girls than in the BF girls at ages 9 and 12 mo but did not differ significantly among the boys. Length tended to be lower in the BF than in the FF infants (P = 0.07). A significant 2-way interaction was encountered for fat-free mass (FFM); it was lower in the BF than in the FF infants at 3 mo. Mean NCHS weight-for-age and weight-for-length z scores differed by age but not by sex or feeding group. Length-for-age z scores differed by age and sex but not by feeding group.


View this table:
TABLE 1. Anthropometry and body composition of children aged 0.5–24 mo1  
Total energy expenditure
Isotope dilution spaces, fractional turnover rates of 18H and 18O, rates of water turnover, and
View this table:
TABLE 2. Isotope dilution spaces, fractional turnover rates of 2H and 18O, and rates of water and carbon dioxide production measured by the doubly labeled water method in children aged 3–24 mo1  
TEE as measured by the DLW method is summarized in Table 3. TEE (in MJ/d) differed by age, sex, and feeding group. TEE was significantly affected by weight, FFM, and FM. Adjusted for weight, TEE still differed by age and feeding group but not by sex. Adjusted for FFM and FM, TEE differed by feeding group but not by age or sex:

DISCUSSION  
In this study, energy requirements during the first 2 y of life were derived from measurements of TEE and energy deposition. The energy requirements of infants and young children should support a rate of growth and body composition consistent with health. The NCHS growth reference was used to evaluate the adequacy of growth of these infants (13). Although the NCHS growth reference has its shortcomings (25), it has been used extensively to compare growth performance of study populations. Although weight velocity (g/d) was higher in FF than in BF infants aged between 3 and 6 mo, and higher in FF than in BF girls aged between 6 and 9 mo (26), the mean NCHS z scores for weight-for-age, length-for-age, and weight-for-length of the BF and FF groups did not differ significantly and were well within 1 SD of the reference mean. The growth performance of these infants was comparable with that of other BF and FF infant populations in whom socioeconomic and environmental constraints would not be expected to limit growth (27, 28). A pooled analysis of growth data on BF infants from the United States, Canada, and Europe showed consistently downward trends after 2–3 mo in NCHS weight-for-age, length-for-age, and weight-for-length z scores to means of -0.5, -0.29, and -0.32, respectively, at age 12 mo (28). In our study, the growth pattern was similar in the BF and FF groups. Growth in both groups was faster than the NCHS reference in the first 3 mo, declined between the ages of 3 and 12 mo, and then plateaued. Length-for-age z scores hovered around 0 for the first 18 mo in both groups and then increased at 24 mo as a result of a disjunction in the reference where 2 databases were merged. Relative to the NCHS reference and compared with other BF and FF study populations, the growth of these children was satisfactory. The assessment of body composition is more problematic because in vivo body-composition measurements have been made in fewer studies of infants and toddlers. The FM of our children peaked at 30% at age 3–6 mo and then decreased to 25% FM in the second year of life (14). Our values for FM and %FM were 9% higher than the Fomon reference (15) but were comparable with the results of recent studies that used total-body electrical conductivity and 18O dilution (29–31).

The body-composition data also were used to compute the energy cost of growth. The energy content of the newly synthesized tissues is theoretically more accurate when the separate costs of protein and fat deposition are taken into account because the composition of weight gain varies with age. The mean energy cost of growth in this study was 20 kJ/g at 3 mo and 10 kJ/g weight gain thereafter. Much of our understanding of the energy cost of growth has been derived from preterm infants or children recovering from malnutrition (32). Typically, the energy cost of growth in these studies range from 10 to 25 kJ/g. On the basis of the changes in body composition of Fomon's term infant reference (15), the energy cost of growth fell from 19 to 8 kJ/g in the first year of life. In practicality, the energy cost of growth is an issue only during the first half of infancy, during which energy deposition contributes significantly to energy requirements. At ages 3 and 6 mo, the energy cost of growth constituted 22% and 6%, respectively, of total energy requirements; thereafter, it contributed negligibly (2–3%) to total energy requirements.

Our TEE values agree with measurements in children aged 3–24 mo from the United Kingdom, the United States, the Netherlands, The Gambia, Mexico, and Peru (4, 11, 29, 33–43). In these studies, mean TEE values were 0.30 MJ•kg-1•d-1 at 3 mo and 0.33 MJ•kg-1•d-1 from 6 mo onward. In the present study, TEE was found to be a function of age, sex, and feeding group. Differences in TEE between ages could be accounted for by weight or FFM and FM. Differences between boys and girls were accounted for by FFM and FM. Together, these factors accounted for 76% of the variability in TEE. The differences in TEE observed between feeding groups, however, were not explained by these factors, or by other plausible factors measured in this study. Motor development, temperament, morbidity, the return of mothers to work, and child-care arrangements did not explain the difference in TEE between feeding groups. Differences in TEE between BF and FF infants were reported previously by us (11) and others (44, 45).

SMR was also a function of sex, age, and feeding group; however, the latter was dependent on age. Significant differences between the BF and FF infants were evident at 3 and 6 mo only. Lower SMRs in BF infants in early infancy confirmed prior observations (33, 46). Weight and length or FFM and FM explained 85% of the variability in SMR. We compared our SMR measurements with BMR predicted from weight and length using the Schofield equation (47). Predicted BMRs were equal to 0.88 SMR at 3–12 mo, 0.93 SMR at 18 mo, and 1.00 SMR at 24 mo. Schofield compiled 300 measurements from Benedict and Talbot (48, 49), Clagett and Hathaway (50), Harris and Benedict (51), and Karlberg (52) to develop predictive models based on weight and length (47). Experimental conditions varied across studies in which indirect calorimetry was used to measure SMR or resting metabolic rate rather than BMR. Benedict performed measurements on sleeping infants 1–1.5 h after a feeding of sweetened water. Clagett's measurements were done while the infants slept or after an early morning feeding. The 94 measurements in the Harris and Benedict study were in infants during the first week of life, when basal metabolism is known to be lower. The 60 infants in Karlberg's series were fasted and sedated with hexobarbital during the measurements. The influence of neonatal age and sedation might explain the lower values predicted by the Schofield equation compared with our values.

Also implicit in this approach to energy requirements is an understanding of what constitutes developmentally appropriate PALs. As expected, PAL increased significantly with age from 1.2 at 3 mo to 1.4 at 24 mo. According to the Bayley PDI, the motor skills of these infants were developmentally on target. The Bayley PDI was not significantly correlated with TEE, PAL, or AEE at any age. We did not detect any significant difference in EE as the children attained milestones such as crawling or walking. Once the coordination and strength are in place to master these skills, the child may be more energetically efficient than during the learning period. We anticipated an effect of infant temperament on PALs, but neither the category scores for activity, biological rhythmicity, initial approach or withdrawal, adaptability, intensity, mood, persistence or attention span, distractibility, and sensory threshold nor the diagnostic temperament clusters were related to TEE, AEE, or PAL. The fact that we did not detect an effect of temperament may have been due to the lower number of infants in the outermost categories of difficult and slow to warm up. This is in contrast with findings by Wells and Davies (53) of a positive correlation between AEE and distress to limitations on the Rothbart Temperament Questionnaire.

Total energy requirements for the children in the present study were a function of age, sex, and feeding group. Naturally, total energy requirements (in MJ/d) increased as the children grew and were higher in boys than in girls; however, weight or FFM and FM accounted for the differences between ages and sexes. The effect of feeding group on energy requirements was apparent throughout the 2 y, primarily because of the higher TEE in the FF than in the BF infants. Energy requirements (in MJ•kg-1•d-1) were 7%, 8%, 9%, 3%, 1%, and 2% higher in the FF than in the BF infants at ages 3, 6, 9, 12, 18, and 24 mo, respectively. Although we did not detect an interaction between feeding group and age, our data strongly suggest that differences in energy requirements between feeding groups diminish beyond the first year of life.

Our estimations of the total energy requirements were slightly lower than those estimated by Prentice et al (4). Their estimates were 0.40, 0.36, 0.35, 0.35, and 0.34 MJ•kg-1•d-1 at ages 3, 6, 9, 12, and 24 mo, respectively. The discrepancies between databases may be attributed to differences in the proportion of BF and FF infants, the estimated energy deposition for growth, or the wide spectrum of nutritional statuses attributable to the inclusion of infants from the United Kingdom, the United States, Peru, and The Gambia.

Total energy requirements of our infants were 80% of the 1985 FAO/WHO/UNU recommendations for energy intake of infants and toddlers. The 1985 FAO/WHO/UNU recommendations were based on observed energy intakes of infants compiled by Whitehead et al (2) from the literature predating 1940 and up to 1980. Modeling of the data indicated a highly significant curvilinear relation between energy intake per kg body weight and age. The authors attributed the sharp fall in energy intake from age 0 to 6 mo to the rapidly decelerating rate of growth and ascribed the rise in energy intake from age 6 to 12 mo to an increase in PAL. Because of the concern that the data represented earlier infant feeding practices, we compiled data published after 1980 (9). Although we did not find evidence of a strong secular trend in energy intakes of infants, the more recent data were 2–15% lower than the 1985 FAO/WHO/UNU recommendations, in part because of the extra 5% allowance added to the recommendations to correct for underestimation of energy intake. In contrast with the 1985 FAO/WHO/UNU recommendations, our data do not display a curvilinear pattern. Energy requirements gradually increased from 0.29 to 0.33 MJ•kg-1•d-1.

In conclusion, energy requirements during the first 2 y of life were estimated from measurements of TEE and energy deposition. Whether these estimations would balance energy expenditure at a PAL consistent with normal development and allow for deposition of tissues at a rate consistent with health is uncertain. Without normative standards, evaluating the growth, body composition, and PAL of young children is judgmental. Remarkable similarity in mean TEE values among studies conducted in the United Kingdom, the United States, the Netherlands, The Gambia, Mexico, and Peru (4, 11, 29, 33–43) lends support to the validity of the data and provides strong evidence that current recommendations for energy intake during the first 2 y of life should be revised.


ACKNOWLEDGMENTS  
We thank the women who participated in this study and acknowledge the contributions of Marilyn Navarrete for subject recruitment; Sopar Seributra and Sandra Kattner for nursing and dietary support; Maurice Puyau, Firoz Vohra, Judy Joo Posada, JoAnn Pratt, Zahira Colon, Kiyoko Usuki, Shide Zhang, and Deborah Roose for technical assistance; Anne Adolph for data management; Leslie Loddeke for editorial review; and Idelle Tapper for secretarial assistance.


REFERENCES  

  1. FAO/WHO/UNU Expert Consultation. Energy and protein requirements. World Health Organ Tech Rep Ser 1985;724:1–206.
  2. Whitehead RG, Paul AA, Cole TJ. A critical analysis of measured food energy intakes during infancy and early childhood in comparison with current international recommendations. J Hum Nutr 1981;35:339–48.
  3. Ferro-Luzzi A, Durnin JVGA. The assessment of human energy intake and expenditure: a critical review of the recent literature. Rome: Food and Agriculture Organization, 1981:9.
  4. Prentice AM, Lucas A, Vasquez-Velasquez L, Davies PSW, Whitehead RG. Are current dietary guidelines for young children a prescription for overfeeding? Lancet 1988;2:1066–9.
  5. Roberts SB, Coward WA, Schlingenseipen KH, Nohria V, Lucas A. Comparison of the doubly labeled water (2H218O) method with indirect calorimetry and a nutrient-balance study for simultaneous determination of energy expenditure, water intake, and metabolizable energy intake in preterm infants. Am J Clin Nutr 1986;44:315–22.
  6. Jones PJH, Winthrop AL, Schoeller DA, et al. Validation of doubly labeled water for assessing energy expenditure in infants. Pediatr Res 1987;21:242–6.
  7. Jensen CL, Butte NF, Wong WW, Moon JK. Determining energy expenditure in preterm infants: comparison of 2H218O method and indirect calorimetry. Am J Physiol 1992;32:R685–92.
  8. Westerterp KR, Lafeber HN, Sulkers EJ, Sauer PJJ. Comparison of short term indirect calorimetry and doubly labeled water method for the assessment of energy expenditure in preterm infants. Biol Neonate 1991;60:75–82.
  9. Butte NF. Energy requirements of infants. Eur J Clin Nutr 1996;50: S24–36.
  10. Torun B, Davies PSW, Livingstone MBE, Paolisso M, Sackett R, Spurr GB. Energy requirements and dietary energy recommendations for children and adolescents 1 to 18 years old. Eur J Clin Nutr 1996;50:S35–81.
  11. Butte NF, Wong WW, Ferlic L, Smith EO. Energy expenditure and deposition of breast-fed and formula-fed infants during early infancy. Pediatr Res 1990;28:631–40.
  12. Carey WB, McDevitt SC. Revision of the infant temperament questionnaire. Pediatrics 1978;61:735–9.
  13. Hamill PVV, Drizd TA, Johnson CL, Reed RB, Roche AF, Moore WM. Physical growth: National Center for Health Statistics percentiles. Am J Clin Nutr 1979;32:607–29.
  14. Butte NF, Hopkinson JM, Wong WW, Smith EO, Ellis KJ. Body composition during the first two years of life: an updated reference. Pediatr Res 2000;47:578–85.
  15. Fomon SJ, Haschke F, Ziegler EE, Nelson SE. Body composition of reference children from birth to age 10 years. Am J Clin Nutr 1982; 35:1169–75.
  16. Ellis KJ, Shypailo RJ. Whole-body potassium measurements independent of body size. In: Ellis KJ, Eastman JD, eds. Human body composition: in vivo methods, models, and assessment. New York: Plenum Press, 1993:371–5.
  17. International Dietary Energy Consulting Group. The doubly-labeled water method for measuring energy expenditure: technical recommendations for use in humans. In: Prentice AM, ed. Vienna: NAHRES-4 International Atomic Energy Agency, 1990.
  18. Wong WW, Clarke LL, Llaurador M, Ferlic L, Klein PD. The use of cotton balls to collect infant urine samples for 2H/1H and 18O/16O isotope ratio measurements. Int J Appl Radiat Isot 1993;44:1125–8.
  19. Wong WW, Lee LS, Klein PD. Deuterium and oxygen-18 measurements on microliter samples of urine, plasma, saliva, and human milk. Am J Clin Nutr 1987;45:905–13.
  20. Wong WW, Clark LL, Llaurador M, Klein PD. A new zinc product for the reduction of water in physiological fluids to hydrogen gas for 2H/1H isotope ratio measurements. Eur J Clin Nutr 1992;46:69–71.
  21. Wong WW, Cochran WJ, Klish WJ, Smith EO, Lee LS, Klein PD. In vivo isotope-fractionation factors and the measurement of deuterium- and oxygen-18-dilution spaces from plasma, urine, saliva, respiratory water vapor, and carbon dioxide. Am J Clin Nutr 1988;47:1–6.
  22. de Weir JB. New methods for calculating metabolic rate with special reference to protein metabolism. J Physiol 1949;109:1–9.
  23. Black AE, Prentice AM, Coward WA. Use of food quotients to predict respiratory quotients for the doubly-labelled water method of measuring energy expenditure. Hum Nutr Clin Nutr 1986;40C:381–91.
  24. Moon JK, Jensen CL, Butte NF. Fast-response whole body indirect calorimeters for infants. J Appl Physiol 1993;74:476–84.
  25. Dibley MJ, Goldsby JB, Staehling NW, Trowbridge FL. Development of normalized curves for the international growth reference: historical and technical considerations. Am J Clin Nutr 1987;46:736–48.
  26. Butte NF, Wong WW, Hopkinson JM, Smith EO, Ellis KJ. Infant feeding mode affects early growth and body composition. Pediatrics (in press).
  27. Victora CG, Morris SS, Barros FC, de Onis M. The NCHS reference and the growth of breast- and bottle-fed infants. J Nutr 1998;128: 1134–8.
  28. Dewey KG, Peerson JM, Brown KH, et al. Growth of breast-fed infants deviates from current reference data: a pooled analysis of US, Canadian, and European data sets. Pediatrics 1995;96:495–503.
  29. de Bruin NC, Degenhart HJ, Gàl S, Westerterp KR, Stijnen T, Visser HKA. Energy utilization and growth in breast-fed and formula-fed infants measured prospectively during the first year of life. Am J Clin Nutr 1998;67:885–96.
  30. Motil KJ, Sheng H-P, Montandon CM, Wong WW. Human milk protein does not limit growth of breast-fed infants. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 1997;24:10–7.
  31. Bellù R, Ortisi MT, Agostoni C, Riva E, Giovannini M. Total body electrical conductivity derived measurement of the body composition of breast- or formula-fed infants at 12 months. Nutr Res 1997;17:23–9.
  32. Butte NF, Wong WW, Garza C. Energy cost of growth during infancy. Proc Nutr Soc 1989;48:303–12.
  33. Wells JCK, Davies PSW. Energy cost of physical activity in twelve week old infants. Am J Hum Biol 1995;7:85–92.
  34. Davies PSW, Ewing G, Lucas A. Energy expenditure in early infancy. Br J Nutr 1989;62:621–9.
  35. Davies PSW, Day JME, Lucas A. Energy expenditure in early infancy and later body fatness. Int J Obes 1991;15:727–31.
  36. Lucas A, Ewing G, Roberts SB, Coward WA. How much energy does the breast fed infant consume and expend? Br Med J 1987;295:75–7.
  37. Roberts SB, Savage J, Coward WA, Chew B, Lucas A. Energy expenditure and intake in infants born to lean and overweight mothers. N Engl J Med 1988;318:461–6.
  38. Vasquez-Velasquez L. Energy expenditure and physical activity of malnourished Gambian infants. Proc Nutr Soc 1988;47:233–9.
  39. Fjeld CR, Schoeller DA. Energy expenditure of malnourished children during catch-up growth. Proc Nutr Soc 1988;47:227–31.
  40. Butte NF, Villalpando S, Wong WW, Flores-Huerta S, Hernandez-Beltran M, Smith EO. Higher total energy expenditure contributes to growth faltering in breast-fed infants living in rural Mexico. J Nutr 1993;123:1028–35.
  41. Davies PSW. Total energy expenditure in young children. Am J Hum Biol 1996;8:183–8.
  42. Stunkard AJ, Berkowitz RI, Stallings VA, Schoeller DA. Energy intake, not energy output, is a determinant of body size in infants. Am J Clin Nutr 1999;69:524–30.
  43. Davies PSW, Wells JCK, Hinds A, Day JME, Laidlaw A. Total energy expenditure in 9 month and 12 month infants. Eur J Clin Nutr 1997;51:249–52.
  44. Davies PSW, Ewing G, Coward WA, Lucas A. Energy metabolism in breast-fed and formula-fed infants. In: Atkinson SA, Hanson LÅ, Chandra RK, eds. Breast-feeding, nutrition, infection and infant growth in developed and emerging countries. St John's, Canada: Arts Biomedical, 1990:521.
  45. Jiang Z, Yan Q, Su Y, et al. Energy expenditure of Chinese infants in Guangdong Province, south China, determined with use of the doubly labeled water method. Am J Clin Nutr 1998;67:1256–64.
  46. Butte NF. Basal metabolism of infants. In: Schürch B, Scrimshaw NS, eds. Activity, energy expenditure and energy requirements of infants and children. Lausanne, Switzerland: Nestlé Foundation, 1989: 117–37.
  47. Schofield WN, Schofield C, James WPT. Basal metabolic rate—review and prediction, together with an annotated bibliography of source material. Hum Nutr Clin Nutr 1985;39C:1–96.
  48. Benedict FG, Talbot FB. The gaseous metabolism of infants with special reference to its relation to pulse-rate and muscular activity. Washington, DC: Carnegie Institute, 1914. (Publication no. 201:168.)
  49. Benedict FG, Talbot FB. Metabolism and growth from birth to puberty. Washington, DC: Carnegie Institute, 1921. (Publication no. 502:213.)
  50. Clagett DO, Hathaway ML. Basal metabolism of normal infants from three to fifteen months of age. Am J Dis Child 1941;62:967–80.
  51. Harris JA, Benedict FG. A biometric study of basal metabolism in man. Washington, DC: Carnegie Institute, 1919. (Publication no. 279:266.)
  52. Karlberg P. Determination of standard energy metabolism (basal metabolism) in normal infants. Acta Paediatr Scand 1952; 41(suppl):151.
  53. Wells JCK, Davies PSW. Relationship between behavior and energy expenditure in 12-week-old infants. Am J Hum Biol 1996;8:465–72.
Received for publication March 10, 2000. Accepted for publication May 30, 2000.


作者: Nancy F Butte
医学百科App—中西医基础知识学习工具
  • 相关内容
  • 近期更新
  • 热文榜
  • 医学百科App—健康测试工具