Literature
首页医源资料库在线期刊美国临床营养学杂志2002年76卷第3期

Higher maternal plasma docosahexaenoic acid during pregnancy is associated with more mature neonatal sleep-state patterning

来源:《美国临床营养学杂志》
摘要:ABSTRACTBackground:Theeffectofdocosahexaenoicacid(DHA)onthedevelopingfetalcentralnervoussystem(CNS)andrelatedfunctionaloutcomesininfancyremainunexplored。Objective:WeinvestigatedwhetherCNSintegrityinnewborns,measuredwithsleeprecordings,wasassociatedwithmatern......

点击显示 收起

Sunita R Cheruku, Hawley E Montgomery-Downs, Susanna L Farkas, Evelyn B Thoman and Carol J Lammi-Keefe

1 From the Department of Nutritional Sciences (SRC, SLF, and CJL-K) and the Biobehavioral Sciences Graduate Program (EBT and HEM-D), the University of Connecticut, Storrs.

2 Represents Scientific Contribution no. 1965, Storrs Agricultural Experiment Station, University of Connecticut, Storrs.

3 Supported by grants from the NIH (HD 32903), the US Department of Agriculture (93-37200-8876), the Donaghue Medical Research Foundation, and the University of Connecticut Research Foundation.

4 Address reprint requests to CJ Lammi-Keefe, Department of Nutritional Sciences, University of Connecticut, U-17, 3624 Horsebarn Road Extension, Storrs, CT 06269-4017. E-mail: clammi{at}canr.uconn.edu.


ABSTRACT  
Background: The effect of docosahexaenoic acid (DHA) on the developing fetal central nervous system (CNS) and related functional outcomes in infancy remain unexplored. Sleep and wake states of newborns provide a tool for assessing the functional integrity of the CNS.

Objective: We investigated whether CNS integrity in newborns, measured with sleep recordings, was associated with maternal concentrations of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids, especially DHA.

Design: Plasma phospholipid fatty acid concentrations were measured in 17 women at parturition. On postpartum day 1 (P1) and day 2 (P2), a pressure-sensitive pad under the infants’ bedding recorded body movements and respiratory patterns to measure sleep and wake states.

Results: Maternal plasma phospholipid DHA ranged from 1.91% to 4.5% by wt of total fatty acids. On the basis of previously published data and the median DHA concentration, the women were divided into 2 groups: high DHA (> 3.0% by wt of total fatty acids) and low DHA ( 3.0% by wt of total fatty acids). Infants of high-DHA mothers had a significantly lower ratio of active sleep (AS) to quiet sleep (QS) and less AS than did infants of low-DHA mothers. Furthermore, the former infants had less sleep-wake transition and more wakefulness on P2. Correlations of maternal DHA status with infant sleep states were consistent with these data. Also, the ratio of maternal n-6 to n-3 fatty acids on P1 was inversely associated with QS and positively associated with arousals in QS. On P2, maternal n-6:n-3 was positively associated with AS, sleep-wake transition, and AS:QS.

Conclusion: The sleep patterns of infants born to mothers with higher plasma phospholipid DHA suggest greater CNS maturity.

Key Words: Long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids • long-chain PUFAs • docosahexaenoic acid • central nervous system • infant sleep • sleep recordings • active sleep • quiet sleep • pregnancy • prenatal nutrition • maternal diet • phospholipids


INTRODUCTION  
The phospholipids of the brain and retina are characterized by high contents of n-3 and n-6 long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids (LCPUFAs), in particular docosahexaenoic acid (DHA; 22:6n-3). An adequate supply of LCPUFAs is necessary for the functional maturation of these tissues during embryonic and neonatal development. The fetus is capable of synthesizing LCPUFAs to some extent, but most of the LCPUFAs are obtained from the maternal circulation (1). During pregnancy, especially the third trimester, LCPUFAs are selectively transferred from the maternal blood to the fetal blood and subsequently to fetal tissues (2–4). After transfer across the placenta, the LCPUFAs are compartmentalized into lipid fractions that do not move back across the placental barrier and therefore are readily available to the developing fetal tissues (5). Thus, fetal accretion of the LCPUFAs is dependent upon maternal LCPUFA status.

The effects of LCPUFAs on visual development are well documented. Several studies showed that higher intakes of LCPUFAs, especially DHA, are associated with enhanced visual development (6, 7). Regarding brain development, suboptimal LCPUFA accretion into the developing central nervous system (CNS) is considered detrimental, because it might affect the child’s emotional and intellectual development (8). However, the effect of fetal DHA status on the developing brain has not been investigated because of the lack of an appropriate assessment technique for use in the neonate. Because the developing nervous system is susceptible to potentially damaging factors of a biochemical or mechanical nature, any assessment technique must focus upon the functional integrity of the developing CNS (9). Measurements of the sleep and wake states of infants provide a tool for assessing the functional integrity of the CNS from the time of birth.

Infants’ sleep patterns are an expression of central integrative control (10). Multiple mechanisms involving both neural and humoral processes in various regions of the brain interact to produce sleep and wakefulness (11). Studies of the relation between sleep-state organization and CNS function showed that changes in sleep architecture are associated with neurologic changes during development and that deviant sleep patterns are associated with neurologic deficits (12, 13).

In the present study, we tested the hypothesis that the integrity of the CNS in newborns, assessed with prolonged sleep recordings, would be associated with maternal LCPUFA status, especially DHA status.


SUBJECTS AND METHODS  
Subjects
Healthy pregnant women (n = 17) were recruited from the Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology at a local hospital after they were admitted for delivery. We excluded women with a history of chronic hypertension, hyperlipidemia, renal or liver disease, heart disease, thyroid disorders, multiple gestations, or pregnancy-induced complications (eg, hypertension, preterm labor, or premature rupture of membranes). Women who were treated during labor with drugs that affect the respiration of newborns, such as magnesium sulfate and butorphanal, were also excluded from the study. Any infants with < 4 h of crib time in the first and second days postpartum were excluded from the study. Mothers were asked about past and current smoking and alcohol use, and this information was recorded. The University of Connecticut Human Subjects Review Committee and the Windham Hospital Institutional Review Board approved the protocol, and written, informed consent was obtained from each subject at the time of recruitment.

Blood sample collection
At delivery, maternal venous blood was collected into tubes containing EDTA. The plasma and erythrocytes were separated with centrifugation (700 x g at 4 °C for 10 min). The plasma was divided into aliquots and stored at -80 °C until analyzed for phospholipid fatty acids. Infant blood samples were not collected because previous studies in our laboratory showed positive associations between maternal PUFA and cord blood PUFA concentrations (14). Thus, maternal PUFA status reflects infant PUFA status. Data regarding mode of delivery, birth weight, head circumference, Apgar scores, and birth trauma were collected from the medical records.

Sleep recordings
The sleep recordings were obtained by using the Motility Monitoring System (MMS) throughout the infant’s stay in the hospital. The basic principle underlying these sleep recordings is that each sleep or wake state involves a unique pattern of motility produced by respiration and body movements. This concept has been validated by comparing MMS data with direct observations and electroencephalogram recordings (15, 16).

The MMS consists of a capacitance-type sensor pad (12 inches x 24 inches x 1/8 inch thick) which is connected to an amplifier leading to a small 24-h data recorder. The sensor pad is placed under the sheet or mattress pad in the infant’s crib. The amplifier and recorder are battery-driven and fit into a briefcase that is suspended on the crib. A single channel of analogue signals from the infant’s respiration and body movements is transmitted from the sensor pad to the amplifier and then digitized (at 10 samples/s) and stored in the data logger. The data are transferred to a computer in the laboratory for processing and analysis. This procedure does not require the presence of an observer and permits continuous recording of data. The crib remains fully mobile during data collection. This method is nonintrusive because the instrumentation is not in direct contact with the infant’s body.

Sample analysis: plasma phospholipid fatty acids
Plasma phospholipid fatty acid concentrations were analyzed with methods described previously (17). In brief, the total lipid was extracted from the maternal plasma by using a modified Folch procedure (18) with 2:1 (vol:vol) dichloromethane:methanol. Thin-layer chromatography was used to separate the phospholipids, which were methylated to form the fatty acid methyl esters. We used 17:0 as an internal standard to determine absolute concentrations in mg/L. Fatty acid methyl esters were separated via injection into a Hewlett-Packard 6890 gas chromatograph (Hewlett-Packard, Wilmington, DE) equipped with an Omegawax 250 capillary column (30 m x 250 mm x 25 µm; Supelco, Bellefonte, PA). Plasma phospholipid fatty acid methyl esters were identified by comparison with external standards and were expressed as both relative % by wt of total fatty acids and absolute concentrations (mg/L).

Analysis of sleep recordings
In the laboratory, the recorded data were entered into a computer and scored by using a pattern-recognition program. Each 30-s period of the recording was scored for the sleep states of quiet sleep (QS), active sleep (AS), sleep-wake transition, wakefulness, and time spent out of the crib.

The computer program that we used for scoring the sleep states from analogue signals was developed on the basis of a template-matching process that compares waveform signals directly. The data profile for each 30-s period in an infant’s record is compared with data profiles from waveform regions that were selected as prototypical for each of the sleep states. The 19 standard templates were chosen from recordings of several infants from other studies, and > 1 template was chosen for each state because waveforms are not homogeneous for any of the states. The templates represent the range of motility patterns that occur in a behavioral state category. Computer assignment of a state to each successive 30-s period in the record is performed by comparing the profile for that period with the profile for each of the state templates in the file. The period is assigned to the state category of the template that it matches most closely. After an infant’s record is scored in 30-s periods, it is smoothed by using the following rule: 6 successive periods of AS, QS, or wakefulness are required for a change of state; thus, when  5 periods of a different state occur, the ongoing state is not changed.

After the computer scoring of each record, the full 24 h of signals is printed out for visual editing. For each 24-h observation, the following measures are obtained: 1) time spent in the crib, 2) percentage of time spent in each of the sleep states (QS, AS, sleep-wake transition, and wakefulness), 3) ratio of AS to QS, 4) brief arousals in AS, and 5) brief arousals in QS. Each of these measures was obtained separately for each infant on postpartum day 1 (P1) and day 2 (P2).

In previous studies, we repeatedly showed measurement reliability and validity for each of the sleep measures obtained from MMS recordings. Reliability of measurement was shown for preterm and full-term infants during the first 6 mo of life (10, 12, 19–22). Validity was shown by making comparisons across species and procedures, including behavioral observations and electroencephalograms (16, 19, 23–25). Predictive validity of the sleep measures was found for newborns (26) and for infants during the early postnatal weeks (12, 23).

Statistical analyses
Simple linear regression analyses were used to describe the associations between maternal LCPUFA status and infant sleep measures, separately on P1 and P2. For maternal DHA concentrations, we divided the women into 2 categories: high DHA (> 3.0% by wt of total fatty acids; n = 10) and low DHA ( 3.0% by wt of total fatty acids; n = 7). The cutoff between the 2 categories was chosen on the basis of our previously published data on plasma phospholipid fatty acids of pregnant women in the third trimester (17) and the median DHA concentration (% by wt of total fatty acids) for this study population. Student’s t tests were used to analyze for significant differences between the group means for the descriptive data (age, maternal education, length of gestation, infant birth weight, birth length, head circumference, and Apgar score). Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to compare the maternal fatty acid profiles of the 2 groups. Repeated-measures ANOVA was used to analyze for the main effects of group and day on the sleep measures, and group-by-day interactions. P < 0.05 was considered statistically significant for the main effects of group and day and P < 0.10 was considered significant for the interaction effect. Furthermore, ANOVA was used to compare individual sleep measures of the 2 groups separately on P1 and P2 for variables with significant group-by-day interactions in the repeated-measures ANOVA. We considered maternal age and maternal education to be possible confounding variables and therefore we used them as covariates in the ANOVAs for group differences in sleep measures. The statistical analyses were performed with SAS, version 6.09 (SAS Institute Inc, Cary, NC).


RESULTS  
Regression analyses used to describe the associations of maternal plasma phospholipid fatty acid concentrations with infant sleep and wake states indicated that, among the n-6 and n-3 LCPUFAs, only the n-3 LCPUFAs, especially DHA, and the n-6:n-3 showed strong correlations (P < 0.05) on both days of measurement. The total n-6 and n-3 LCPUFA concentrations were not related to infant sleep measures.

The following correlations were the most significant among all the statistically significant correlations for this population. On P2, maternal DHA concentration was negatively associated with AS (r = -0.49, P < 0.05), AS:QS (r = -0.55, P < 0.05), and sleep-wake transition (r = -0.49, P < 0.05) and positively associated with wakefulness (r = 0.51, P < 0.05) (Figure 1). On P1, the ratio of n-6 to n-3 LCPUFAs in maternal plasma was negatively associated with QS (r = -0.47, P = 0.05) and positively associated with arousals in QS (r = 0.51, P < 0.05) (Figure 2). On P2, the ratio of n-6 to n-3 LCPUFAs in maternal plasma was positively associated with AS (r = 0.53, P < 0.05), AS:QS (r = 0.63, P < 0.01), and sleep-wake transition (r = 0.52, P < 0.05) and negatively associated with wakefulness (r = -0.53, P < 0.05) (Figure 2).


View larger version (20K):
FIGURE 1. . Associations of maternal plasma phospholipid docosahexaenoic acid (DHA) concentrations with infant sleep states on postpartum day 2; n = 17. Sleep states were measured as the percentage of time that the infant spent in that sleep state. AS, active sleep; QS, quiet sleep.

 

View larger version (30K):
FIGURE 2. . Associations of the maternal plasma ratio of n-6 to n-3 long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids with infant sleep states on postpartum day 1 (P1) and day 2 (P2); n = 17. Sleep states were measured as the percentage of time that the infant spent in that sleep state. AS, active sleep; QS, quiet sleep.

 
Maternal and infant characteristics of the high-DHA and low-DHA groups are shown in Table 1. There were no significant differences between the 2 groups.


View this table:
TABLE 1 . Maternal and infant characteristics of the groups with high or low maternal docosahexaenoic acid (DHA) concentrations1  
Plasma phospholipid LCPUFA concentrations of the women in the high-DHA and low-DHA groups are shown in Table 2. Maternal plasma phospholipid DHA concentrations ranged from 1.91% to 4.5% by wt of total fatty acids. Women in the high-DHA group had significantly higher concentrations of dihomo--linolenic acid (20:3n-6), osbond acid (22:5n-6), eicosapentaenoic acid (20:5n-3), docosapentaenoic acid (22:5n-3), DHA, n-3 PUFAs, and n-3 LCPUFAs. Also, women in the high-DHA group had a significantly lower ratio of total n-6 LCPUFAs to total n-3 LCPUFAs compared with the low-DHA group. Concentrations of mead acid (20:3n-9), the most unsaturated and elongated of the n-9 fatty acids, did not differ significantly between the high-DHA and low-DHA groups.


View this table:
TABLE 2 . Maternal plasma phospholipid long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid (LCPUFA) concentrations in the groups with high or low maternal docosahexaenoic acid (DHA) concentrations1  
The mean (± SD) values for the sleep measures in the high-DHA and low-DHA groups on days P1 and P2 are shown in Table 3. The values for the different sleep measures represent the percentage of time spent in the crib in that particular sleep state. These data were analyzed with ANOVA, with group at 2 levels (high- and low-DHA) and day as the repeated measure. The group effect was significant (P < 0.05) for AS and AS:QS. For QS, the group effect was nearly significant (P = 0.06). For the variables wakefulness and sleep-wake transition, the group-by-day interaction was significant (P < 0.10), so we performed simple effect testing to determine whether the high-DHA and low-DHA groups differed from each other within days (Table 3). Infants of mothers with high DHA concentrations had significantly less AS and had a lower AS:QS compared with infants of mothers with low DHA concentrations. Furthermore, infants in the high-DHA group had significantly less sleep-wake transition and more wakefulness than did infants in the low-DHA group on P2.


View this table:
TABLE 3 . Sleep measures of the infants in the groups with high or low maternal docosahexaenoic acid (DHA) concentrations on postnatal day 1 (P1) and day 2 (P2)1  
The high-DHA and low-DHA groups differed with regard to maternal age and education, and although these differences were not statistically significant in this sample, we considered the possibility that they might be operating as confounding variables. However, none of the results discussed above and shown in Table 3 changed when we included maternal age and maternal education as covariates in the ANOVAs for group differences in sleep measures.


DISCUSSION  
An inadequate supply of LCPUFAs during development may be a causal factor in certain neurologic disorders (27, 28) and also in behavioral and other functional deficits (29–31). During the third trimester of pregnancy, n-6 and n-3 fatty acids accrue in the fetal tissues and rapid synthesis of brain tissue occurs. The rapid growth of brain tissue causes increases in cell number, cell size, and cell type. It is critical for the developing fetus to obtain the correct types and amounts of fatty acids to ensure complete and proper development of the brain (32).

Animal studies of n-3 deficiencies, especially DHA deficits, have provided the basis for studying the effects of different amounts of DHA on the functional status of human infants (33). Most of the human studies have focused on the effects of different amounts of DHA in the infant’s diet (ie, breast milk containing LCPUFAs compared with formulas with and without LCPUFAs) on postnatal CNS development (34, 35). Lucas et al (35) showed that infant neurodevelopment is dependent on factors that are present in breast milk but not in formula. Although definitive evidence supporting the association is lacking, these factors include LCPUFAs, specifically DHA, which is recognized as necessary for maturation of the CNS (36). Because neuronal proliferation and synaptogenesis begin late in gestation and continue into postnatal life (37), it is crucial to study the factors that affect prenatal and postnatal brain development. The prenatal supply of DHA is one such factor and is mostly dependent on maternal DHA status (17).

Several studies suggest that intakes of essential fatty acids may modulate sleep (38–40). Malikova et al (38) reported that changes in the ratio of n-3 to n-6 fatty acids in the diet of guinea pigs affected the total sleeping time. Fagioli et al (39) characterized the effects of long-term deficiency of essential fatty acids on sleep organization by studying children who were maintained on fat-free total parenteral nutrition for 2–6 mo. These children had reduced amounts of slow-wave sleep compared with children who received total parenteral nutrition with essential fatty acids. These results suggest that intakes of essential fatty acids may modulate sleep (39). Furthermore, it was observed that breast-fed infants had a more mature pattern of CNS development than did formula-fed infants. The breast-fed infants had less AS and longer periods of QS (40). Our findings are consistent with these findings.

In a study of premature infants, Thoman and Whitney (12) found significantly different, distinctive sleep patterns in 4 groups of infants: those who showed normal development and those with a minimal mental deficiency, a neurologic disorder, or physical dysfunction. None of these infants had been diagnosed with an abnormality at the time of the early sleep recordings. In another study, Freudigman and Thoman (13) reported that a newborn’s sleep characteristics were related to mental and motor development at 6 mo. Thus, sleep recordings in early infancy provide a uniquely sensitive indication of subtle prenatal compromise, which may not have any apparent effects until a later age.

The present study had limitations in that it was a correlational analysis with a descriptive, nonrandomized design. However, the results provide evidence that prenatal exposure to higher concentrations of DHA results in a more mature pattern of sleep and wake states. QS, as a percentage of total sleep, increases from the time of birth until it is predominant at 3 mo postterm and thereafter (41). It is well documented that the increase in QS with age is largely a function of maturation of the nervous system and is not environmental (42). Thus, in our study, the lower amounts of AS and the greater amounts of QS observed in the infants exposed prenatally to higher DHA concentrations suggest greater CNS maturity compared with the infants exposed to lower DHA concentrations. Furthermore, the lower AS:QS observed in the infants in the high-DHA group shows that their sleep organization soon after birth was approaching that of normal, older infants. Generally, consolidation of sleep increases with age, and therefore less sleep-wake transition and more wakefulness in the infants in the high-DHA group also reflects greater maturity.

In addition, the ratio of total n-6 to n-3 fatty acids significantly influenced the integrity of the CNS. Prenatal exposure to a higher ratio of n-6 LCPUFAs to n-3 LCPUFAs resulted in sleep patterns that were contrary to those seen with a lower ratio of n-6 LCPUFAs to n-3 LCPUFAs and higher DHA concentrations. The higher ratio of n-6 to n-3 fatty acids was associated with more AS, less QS, more sleep-wake transition, less wakefulness, and a higher AS:QS. Furthermore, there was a highly significant positive association between the ratio of n-6 to n-3 fatty acids and arousals in QS. Arousals in QS are defined as QS fragmentation and are associated with CNS vulnerability (13). Future studies should consider maternal nutrition, health, socioeconomic status, and other related factors as potential correlates of DHA status.

The marked differences between the sleep measures on P1 and P2 were expected on the basis of our previous studies (13, 26). Research has shown that the characteristic changes in sleep patterns over the first 2 d postpartum, as seen in this study, occur in several other species. These changes are not only found in human newborns but also in newborn rats and rabbits (24, 25).

In conclusion, our findings suggest that differences in the prenatal supply of LCPUFAs, especially DHA, may modify brain phospholipids and affect neural function. Furthermore, the MMS is a reliable instrument for studying the CNS maturity of an individual infant in relation to infant DHA status. To our knowledge, this is the first report documenting that maternal DHA status during pregnancy, which is significantly influenced by dietary DHA, is associated with CNS maturity of the infant at birth.


ACKNOWLEDGMENTS  
We acknowledge the support of the Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology and the Clinical Laboratory at Windham Hospital, Windham, CT. We thank Mary Fine for her assistance with scoring the sleep states of the infants and Connie Cantor for her technical assistance. We appreciate the cooperation of the subjects in this study.


REFERENCES  

  1. Campbell C, Gordon M, Dutta Roy A. Preferential uptake of LCPUFA by isolated human placental membrane. Mol Cell Biochem 1996;155:77–83.
  2. Friedman Z, Dannon A, Lambert E, Mann W. Cord blood fatty acid composition in infants and in their mothers in the third trimester. J Pediatr 1978;92:461–6.
  3. Ruyle M, Connor WE, Anderson GJ. Placental transfer of EFA in humans: venous-arterial difference for DHA in fetal umbilical erythrocytes. J Natl Acad Sci 1990;87:7902–6.
  4. Wijendran V. Long chain polyunsaturated fatty acid metabolism during normal pregnancy and pregnancy complicated with gestational diabetes mellitus. PhD dissertation. University of Connecticut, Storrs, 1998.
  5. Coleman RA. The role of the placenta in lipid metabolism and transport. Semin Perinatol 1989;13:180–91.
  6. Stockard JE, Saste MD, Benford VJ, Barness L, Austead N, Carver JD. Effect of docosahexaenoic acid content of maternal diet on auditory brain stem conduction times in rat pups. Dev Neurosci 2000;22:494–9.
  7. Neuringer M. Infant vision and retinal function in studies of dietary long chain polyunsaturated fatty acids: methods, results and implications. Am J Clin Nutr 2000;71(suppl):256S–67S.
  8. Neuringer M, Reisbick S, Janowsky J. The role of n-3 fatty acids in visual and cognitive development: current evidence and methods of assessment. J Pediatr 1994;125:S39–47.
  9. Prechtl HFR. Assessment methods for the newborn infant, a critical evaluation. In: Stralton P, ed. Pyschobiology of the human newborn. New York: Wiley & Sons, 1982:1–52.
  10. Thoman EB, Whitney MP. Sleep states of infants monitored in the home: individual differences, developmental trends and origins of diurnal cyclicity. Infant Behav Dev 1989;12:59–75.
  11. Thoman EB, Ingersoll EW. Sleep/wake states of preterm infants: stability, developmental change, diurnal variation, and relation with caregiving activity. Child Dev 1999;70:1–10.
  12. Thoman EB, Whitney MP. Early sleep patterns of premature infants are differentially related to later developmental disabilities. J Dev Behav Pediatr 1993;14:71–80.
  13. Freudigman KA, Thoman EB. Infant sleep during the first postnatal day: an opportunity for assessment of vulnerability. Pediatrics 1993;92:373–9.
  14. Wijendran V, Couch S, Bendel R, Cheruku S, Lammi-Keefe CJ. Transfer of long chain polyunsaturated fatty acids is altered in pregnancy complicated with gestational diabetes mellitus. Lipids 2000;35:927–31.
  15. Thoman EB, Tynan DW. Sleep states and wakefulness in human infants: profiles from motility monitoring. Physiol Behav 1979;23:519–25.
  16. Zeidner LW, Denenberg VH, Thoman EB, Weyand T. Comparisons of behavioral, motoric, and electrical criteria for assessment of sleep wake states in the rabbit. Physiol Behav 1983;31:273–8.
  17. Wijendran V, Bendel R, Couch SC, et al. Maternal plasma phospholipid polyunsaturated fatty acids in pregnancy with and without gestational diabetes mellitus: relations with maternal factors. Am J Clin Nutr 1999;70:53–61.
  18. Folch J, Lees M, Stanley GHS. A simple method for the isolation and purification of total lipids from animal tissues. J Biol Chem 1957;226:497–509.
  19. Thoman EB, Glazier RC. Computer scoring of motility patterns and states of sleep and wakefulness: human infants. Sleep 1987;10:122–9.
  20. Thoman EB, McDowell K. Sleep cyclicity in infants during the earliest postnatal weeks. Physiol Behav 1989;45:517–22.
  21. Thoman EB, Zeidner LP. Sleep-wake states in infant rabbits: profiles from motility monitoring. Physiol Behav 1979;22:1049–54.
  22. Whitney MP, Thoman EB. Sleep in premature and fullterm infants from 24 hour home recordings. Infant Behav Dev 1994;17:223–34.
  23. Thoman EB, Denenberg VH, Sievel J, Zeidner L, Becker PT. State organization in neonates: developmental inconsistency indicates risk for developmental dysfunction. Neuropediatrics 1981;12:45–54.
  24. Caroll DA, Denenberg VH, Thoman EB. Reliability and validity of computer scoring of behavioral sleep-wake states in rats and rabbits. Physiol Behav 1993;54:269–73.
  25. Caroll DA, Denenberg VH, Thoman EB. A comparitive study of quiet sleep, active sleep and waking on the first two days of life. Dev Psychobiol 1999;35:43–8.
  26. Freudigman KA, Thoman EB. Ultradian and diurnal cyclicity in the sleep states of newborn infants during the first two postnatal days. Early Hum Dev 1994;38:67–80.
  27. Rathbone L. The effect of diet on the fatty acid composition of serum, brain, brain mitochondria and myelin in the rat. Biochem J 1965;97:620–8.
  28. Clausen J, Moller J. Experimental allergic encephalomyelitis provoked in rats after lack of polyunsaturated fatty acids. Acta Neurol Scand 1967;43:375–88.
  29. Yamamoto N, Hashimoto A, Takemoto Y. Effect of the dietary 18:2n-6/18:3n-3 balance on lipid composition and learning ability of rats. II. Discrimination process, extinction process and glycolipid composition. J Lipid Res 1988;28:144–51.
  30. Lamptey MS, Walker BL. A possible essential role for dietary linolenic acid in the development of the young rat. J Nutr 1976;106:86–93.
  31. Bourre LM, Francois M, Youyou A. The effects of dietary 18:3n-3 on the composition of nerve membranes, enzymatic activity, amplitude of electrophysiological parameters, resistance to poisons and performance of learning tasks in rats. J Nutr 1982;119:1880–92.
  32. Clandinin MT, Chappell JE, Leong S, Heim T, Swyer PR, Chance GW. Intrauterine fatty acid accretion rates in human brain: implications for fatty acid requirements. Early Hum Dev 1980;4:121–9.
  33. Reisbick S, Neuringer M, Gohl E, Wald R. Visual attention in infant monkeys: effects of dietary fatty acids and age. Dev Psychol 1997;33:387–95.
  34. Willats P, Forsyth JS, DiModugrio MK, Varma S, Colvin M. Effect of long chain polyunsaturated fatty acids in infant formula on problem solving at 10 months of age. Lancet 1998,352:688–91.
  35. Lucas A, Morley R, Cole TJ, Lister G, Leeson-Payne C. Breast milk and subsequent intelligence quotient in children born preterm. Lancet 1992,339:261–4.
  36. Carlson S. Long chain polyunsaturated fatty acids and development of human infants. Acta Paediatr 1999;88:72–7.
  37. Huttenlocher P, deCourten CH. The development of synapses in the striate cortex of man. Hum Neurobiol 1987;6:1–9.
  38. Malikova NA, Krzhechkovskasia VV, Marokko IN, Mazo VK. The effect of fatty compounds with different ratio of the polyunsaturated fatty acids of the omega-3 and omega-6 families on the expression of food related anaphylaxis, on the liver cytochrome p-450 system and on 17-hydroxycorticosteroid metabolism in guinea pigs. Vopr Pitan 1995;4:13–6.
  39. Fagioli I, Barconcini P, Ricour C, Salzarulo P. Decrease of slow wave sleep in children with prolonged absence of essential lipids intake. Sleep 1989;12:495–9.
  40. Uauy R, Peirano P, Hoffman D, Mena P, Birch D, Birch E. Role of essential fatty acids in the function of the developing nervous system. Lipids 1996;31:167S–76S.
  41. Roffwarg HP, Muzio JN. Ontogenic development of the human sleep-dream cycle. Science 1966;152:604–19.
  42. Stern E, Parmelee AH, Akiyama Y, Schultz MA, Wenner WH. Sleep cycle characteristics in infants. Pediatrics 1969;43:65–70.
Received for publication July 10, 2001. Accepted for publication September 24, 2001.


作者: Sunita R Cheruku
医学百科App—中西医基础知识学习工具
  • 相关内容
  • 近期更新
  • 热文榜
  • 医学百科App—健康测试工具