Literature
首页医源资料库在线期刊美国临床营养学杂志2003年78卷第3期

Homocysteine and cognitive function in the Sacramento Area Latino Study on Aging

来源:《美国临床营养学杂志》
摘要:Room3200A,4645SecondAvenue,Sacramento,CA95817。ABSTRACTBackground:Elevatedplasmahomocysteine(hyperhomocysteinemia),anindependentriskfactorforvasculardisease,hasbeenreportedtobeinverselycorrelatedwithobjectivemeasuresofcognitivefunctioninpatientswithAlzheimerdi......

点击显示 收起

Joshua W Miller, Ralph Green, Marisa I Ramos, Lindsay H Allen, Dan M Mungas, William J Jagust and Mary N Haan

1 From the Departments of Medical Pathology (JWM, RG, and MIR) and Neurology (DMM and WJJ), School of Medicine, and the Department of Nutrition (LHA), University of California, Davis, and the Department of Epidemiology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor (MNH).

2 Presented in part at the Experimental Biology annual meetings for 1999 (Washington, DC, 17–21 April 1999) and 2000 (San Diego, 15–18 April 2000) and published in abstract form (FASEB J 1999;13:A374 and FASEB J 2000;14:A256).

3 Supported by grants RO1 AG12975 and AG10129 from the NIH and grant 5456 from the US Department of Agriculture.

4 Address reprint requests to JW Miller, UC Davis Medical Center, Department of Medical Pathology, Research III–Room 3200A, 4645 Second Avenue, Sacramento, CA 95817. E-mail: jwmiller{at}ucdavis.edu.

See corresponding editorial on page 359.


ABSTRACT  
Background: Elevated plasma homocysteine (hyperhomocysteinemia), an independent risk factor for vascular disease, has been reported to be inversely correlated with objective measures of cognitive function in patients with Alzheimer disease and in community-dwelling older adults.

Objective: We evaluated the cross-sectional relation between total plasma homocysteine concentration and cognitive function in elderly Latinos (aged =" BORDER="0"> 60 y; n = 1789) participating in the Sacramento Area Latino Study on Aging.

Design: Global cognitive function was assessed by using the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination, and specific cognitive functions were assessed by using 6 instruments developed for cross-cultural and multilingual neuropsychological evaluation of older persons. Associations between the cognitive function scores and total plasma homocysteine concentrations were then measured by multiple regression analysis with control for potential confounding by nutrient status (red blood cell folate, plasma vitamin B-12), kidney function (serum creatinine), demographic variables (age, sex, education, acculturation), and depressive symptoms.

Results: Modest inverse associations were found between homocysteine concentrations and several indexes of cognitive function, including the global Modified Mini-Mental State Examination assessment and the picture-association, verbal attention-span, and pattern-recognition tests (P 0.05). Demographic variables, particularly age and education, were more strongly associated with cognitive function scores than was homocysteine.

Conclusions: Homocysteine is a modest independent predictor of cognitive function in community-dwelling elderly Latinos. Reducing plasma homocysteine concentrations by administering B-vitamin supplements may provide some protection against cognitive decline in this and other elderly populations, but the effect may be limited.

Key Words: Homocysteine • cognitive function • Modified Mini-Mental State Examination • folate • vitamin B-12 • creatinine • elderly • aging • Latinos


INTRODUCTION  
Vascular disease and its risk factors are receiving increasing attention as potentially modifiable causes of cognitive decline and dementia in older adults. Stroke, cardiovascular disease, peripheral vascular disease, hypertension, and diabetes mellitus have each been associated with cognitive deficits or dementia, or both, in various elderly populations (1–5).

An elevated plasma concentration of the sulfur amino acid homocysteine (hyperhomocysteinemia) is now recognized as an independent risk factor for cardiovascular, peripheral vascular, and cerebrovascular disease (6). Accordingly, a potential influence of hyperhomocysteinemia on cognitive functioning and dementia has been postulated. Clarke et al (7) compared patients with histologically confirmed Alzheimer disease with age-matched healthy control subjects. They found that individuals who had elevated serum homocysteine concentrations (> 14 µmol/L) were 4.5 times as likely to have had Alzheimer disease (as confirmed by post-mortem histologic analysis) as were those with low serum homocysteine ( 11 µmol/L). Similar observations have been made in other studies (8, 9), including the recent finding that baseline homocysteine concentrations predicted the risk of incident Alzheimer disease and dementia over an 8-y period in the Framingham Heart Study population (10). According to findings we published recently, however, it remains unclear whether elevated homocysteine is a causative factor or simply a marker of coexistent vascular disease in Alzheimer disease patients (11). Nonetheless, in addition to correlations with Alzheimer disease and dementia, significant correlations have been observed between homocysteine concentrations and indexes of cognitive function in case-control studies of patients with psychogeriatric conditions (12–15) and in cross-sectional, population-based studies of community-dwelling older adults (16–19). Two studies, however, found no significant associations between homocysteine concentrations and cognitive function in either cross-sectional (20, 21) or longitudinal (20) analyses.

The purpose of the present study was to investigate the association between homocysteine concentrations and cognitive function in the Sacramento Area Latino Study on Aging (SALSA), a community-based cohort study of elderly Latinos (aged =" BORDER="0"> 60 y).


SUBJECTS AND METHODS  
Subjects
Subject recruitment and study procedures were approved by the Human Subjects Review Committee at the University of California, Davis, and written informed consent was obtained from all study participants. The study population consisted of community-dwelling older adults (aged =" BORDER="0"> 60 y; n = 1789) of Latino ancestry residing in Sacramento, CA, and the surrounding Northern California communities. Subjects were considered "Latino" if they, their parents, or their grandparents were born in Central or South America. Subjects were recruited over a period of 18 mo beginning in February 1998. The details of sampling and recruitment were described elsewhere (22). Briefly, 1990 census tracts and more recent sampling lists from other sources were used to identify localities in which =" BORDER="0"> 5% of the population was potentially eligible for the study on the basis of Latino ancestry and age. Subjects were recruited by a variety of means, including mailings, telephone calls, and door-to-door canvassing, and 82% of those contacted agreed to participate. The age and sex distributions of the final sample compared favorably with data collected during a 1998 Sacramento-area dress rehearsal for the Year 2000 United States Census, and thus the final sample was generally representative of the target population. Subject recruitment and study procedures were approved by the Human Subjects Review Committee at the University of California, Davis, and written informed consent was obtained from all study participants.

Sample collection and analysis
Fasting blood was collected from each participant by standard venipuncture into evacuated tubes with and without EDTA. The blood was transported on ice to the Medical Center Clinical Laboratory at the University of California, Davis, for processing within 4 h of collection. Plasma and serum were isolated and stored at -80 °C until they were analyzed. Plasma homocysteine concentrations were measured by using HPLC with post-column fluorescence detection (23). Red blood cell (RBC) folate was measured by using an automated chemiluminescence assay [ACS 180; Chiron Diagnostics (now Bayer Diagnostics), Tarrytown, NY). Plasma vitamin B-12 concentrations were measured by using a radioassay (Quantaphase II; BioRad Diagnostics, Hercules, CA). Serum creatinine was analyzed by using a standard spectrophotometric assay. Because the present study was a substudy of the larger SALSA, there was significant demand for blood samples to be used for a variety of other biochemical assays, and sufficient volumes of blood were not available from all subjects for each assay. The sample sizes for the study of each of the biochemical variables were n = 1642, 1501, 1545, and 1640 for plasma homocysteine, RBC folate, plasma vitamin B-12, and serum creatinine, respectively.

Neuropsychological assessment instruments
Neuropsychological assessments were made with the use of several instruments. The Modified Mini-Mental State Examination (3MSE; 24), which evaluates memory, orientation, attention, and language on a scale of 0–100, was used to determine global cognitive ability. Specific subdomains of cognitive function were assessed by using a battery of 6 instruments developed for cross-cultural and multilingual neuropsychological assessment of older persons (25). These instruments are summarized here.

Delayed recall
The delayed-recall scale is an assessment of the ability to learn and recall verbal information. This test follows a standard word-list learning-task format using an inventory of 15 common items that can be purchased in a grocery store. The list contains words from 5 semantic categories with various numbers of exemplars. Subjects are put through 5 learning trials and a delayed-recall trial, with the order of presentation fixed across the trials. Scores are based on a 15-point scale.

Object naming
The object-naming scale is an assessment of the ability to retrieve verbal information from semantic memory. Subjects are shown color pictures and are asked to name specific objects. Scores are based on a 20-point scale.

Picture association
The picture-association scale is an assessment of the ability to access and use semantic memory of nonverbal information. Subjects are shown color pictures of stimulus objects along with 6–12 potentially associated images, and they are asked to point to the one image that is most related to the stimulus. Scores are based on a 20-point scale.

Verbal conceptual thinking
The verbal conceptual-thinking scale is an assessment of the capacity for abstract or conceptual thinking. The subject is presented with 6 words, 5 of which belong to a common category, and is asked to identify the outlier. Scores are based on a 20-point scale.

Verbal attention span
The verbal attention-span scale is an assessment of fixed attention span. Subjects are asked to repeat a string of digits read at a rate of 1/s. Some items contain nonrandom sequences of digits, which are included to facilitate grouping of information and produce subtle gradients of item difficulty. Scores are based on a 20-point scale.

Pattern recognition
The pattern-recognition scale is an assessment of the ability to discriminate between black and white designs. A stimulus design is presented with 6 target alternatives, one of which is identical to the stimulus, and the subject must indicate which target is the same as the stimulus. Scores are based on a 20-point scale.

Demographic data
The 3MSE and delayed-recall instruments were administered to all but 10 subjects (n = 1779). The other 5 neuropsychological instruments were administered to a subgroup of the total population (n = 537) consisting of all subjects who scored below the 20th percentile on either the 3MSE or delayed-recall instruments and a random sample of 20% of the total population.

Subjects were given the choice of taking the neuropsychological tests in English or Spanish. Age, sex, and the number of years of education were recorded for each subject as potential determinants of cognitive function scores. In addition, an acculturation score was determined by using the Acculturation Rating Scale for Mexican Americans–II (26). This is a self-reported measure of how well an individual is assimilated into American culture as determined by such factors as language preference, food choices, entertainment choices, and social group preference. Individuals were scored on a scale of 0–37; a score of 0 indicated no acculturation, and a score of 37 indicated complete acculturation. Depressive symptoms were assessed with the use of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression scale (CES-D; 27).

Statistical analysis
Associations between plasma homocysteine concentrations (dependent variable) and each of the demographic (ie, age, sex, education, and acculturation), biochemical (ie, RBC folate, plasma vitamin B-12, and serum creatinine), and depressive symptom (CES-D) variables were evaluated by multiple linear regression analysis. Multiple linear regression analyses were also used to build statistical models describing the relations between homocysteine concentration (independent variable) and each of the cognitive-function test instruments (dependent variables) before and after adjustment for confounding by the demographic, biochemical, and depressive symptom variables. A general linear model procedure and the Tukey-Kramer multiple-comparisons test were used to compare mean ± SEM cognitive function scores among subjects grouped by quintiles of homocysteine concentrations, by age, and by education. Because the values for plasma homocysteine, RBC folate, plasma vitamin B-12, and serum creatinine were not normally distributed (ie, there was tailing toward higher values), these variables were natural log-transformed before analysis. Statistical significance was defined for all analyses as P < 0.05. The statistical analyses were carried out by using STATVIEW for MACINTOSH and WINDOWS (version 5.0.1; Abacus Concept, Berkeley, CA) and SAS for WINDOWS (version 7; SAS Institute Inc, Cary, NC).


RESULTS  
Summary data for the SALSA population are presented in Table 1. A significant proportion (17%) of the population had plasma homocysteine concentrations above the cutoff value for hyperhomocysteinemia defined in a report from the Framingham Heart Study, ie, =" BORDER="0"> 13 µmol/L (28). In the present study, hyperhomocysteinemia had a high prevalence despite the fact that < 1% of the population had RBC folate values below the cutoff for deficiency (ie, 160 ng/mL). Low vitamin B-12 status (plasma vitamin B-12: 200 pg/mL in 6.5% of subjects, 200–300 pg/mL in 16.4%) and impaired renal function (serum creatinine: > 1.4 mg/dL in 5.4% of men and > 1.1 mg/dL in 6.5% of women) were highly prevalent, findings that are consistent with the advanced age of the population. Approximately one-fifth of the population had cognitive function scores indicative of cognitive impairment (3MSE 78, delayed recall 6, or both). One-quarter of the population had elevated depressive symptoms (CES-D: =" BORDER="0"> 16), as previously reported (27). With the use of multiple regression analysis (Table 2), the primary determinants of plasma homocysteine were creatinine, vitamin B-12, and RBC folate; age, sex, education, and acculturation were statistically significant but comparatively minor correlates. The CES-D score was not associated with plasma homocysteine (data not shown).


View this table:
TABLE 1 . Description of the SALSA population1  

View this table:
TABLE 2 . Predictors of plasma homocysteine in the SALSA population1  
A series of 4 regression models describing the relations between homocysteine and cognitive function scores are presented in Table 3. Before adjustment for confounding by demographic and biochemical variables (model 1), homocysteine was inversely correlated with all 7 cognitive function tests (P < 0.001). The R2 values for these simple regressions indicate that homocysteine explains 2.2%–5.1% (R2 = 0.022–0.051) of the variance in cognitive function scores within the sample. Homocysteine remained inversely correlated with all 7 cognitive function tests after the addition of folate, vitamin B-12, and creatinine to the model (in all cases, P < 0.001) (model 2). The inclusion of the demographic variables (ie, age, sex, education, and acculturation) in the model, however, significantly attenuated the relations between homocysteine and the cognitive function tests (models 3 and 4). Correlation coefficients for homocysteine were largely reduced in these models compared with model 1, whereas R2 values were 5-fold–20-fold those in model 1. These R2 values indicate that the demographic variables explain a much higher proportion of the variance in cognitive function scores than do the biochemical variables homocysteine, folate, vitamin B-12, or creatinine. Of the demographic variables, education was the most strongly associated, age and acculturation ranked next, and sex was mostly insignificant (data not shown). In model 4, which included all demographic and biochemical variables, homocysteine remained significantly correlated with 4 of the 7 cognitive function tests (3MSE, picture association, verbal attention, and pattern recognition). Inclusion of the biochemical measures added very little to the variance explained by the demographic factors. The CES-D score did not significantly affect the observed associations between homocysteine and cognitive function scores (data not shown), and therefore it was not included in any of the models.


View this table:
TABLE 3 . Multiple linear regression models for homocysteine (independent variable) versus cognitive function score (dependent variable)1  
The coefficients listed in Table 3 indicate the magnitude of the change in cognitive function score associated with a 1-unit change in the natural log of homocysteine. Such a small change in a natural log of a variable, however, is difficult to interpret. To obtain additional information about the magnitude of the relation, we compared mean ± SEM cognitive function scores, adjusted for the demographic and biochemical variables, among subjects by quintiles of homocysteine concentrations within the sample (Figure 1). The adjusted mean 3MSE score was 1.2 points lower in the highest homocysteine quintile than in the lowest quintile. This difference is not statistically significant by general linear model analysis. Similarly small, nonsignificant differences in adjusted mean scores were observed between the lowest and highest homocysteine quintiles for the 6 other cognitive function tests (data not shown). These data are contrasted with the results shown in Figures 2 and 3, in which adjusted mean 3MSE scores are compared among subjects grouped by age and education within the study sample. The adjusted mean 3MSE score was 5.3 points lower in the oldest subjects (aged =" BORDER="0"> 80 y) than in the youngest subjects (aged 60–69 y) and was 10.8 points higher in those subjects with the greatest number of years of education (> 12 y, ie, beyond high school) than in those with the fewest years of education (0–5 y, ie, elementary school or less). General linear model analyses with post hoc comparisons by the Tukey-Kramer procedure indicate that these differences are statistically significant (P < 0.001). Statistically significant differences in adjusted mean scores also were observed between the lowest and highest age and education groups for the 6 other cognitive function tests (data not shown). In secondary multiple regression analyses, we assessed whether the relative strengths of the relations between homocysteine and cognitive function scores varied among the defined age and education groups. The question was whether homocysteine was more strongly correlated with cognitive function scores in one age or education group [eg, the oldest (aged =" BORDER="0"> 80 y) or the least educated (0–5 y of school) subjects] than with those in another group [eg, the youngest (aged 60–79 y) or the most educated (> 12 y of school) subjects]. No clear differences among the 3 age groups or among the 4 education groups were detected in the strengths of the associations between homocysteine and the cognitive function tests (data not shown). The sample size and power of the study may not have been sufficient to allow us adequately to address this question, however.


View larger version (12K):
FIGURE 1. . Mean (± SEM) scores (0–100) on the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination (3MSE) for subjects in the homocysteine concentration quintiles (Qs), showing the relation between global cognitive function score and homocysteine concentration. The scores were adjusted for folate, vitamin B-12, creatinine, age, sex, education, and acculturation of subjects. n = 1406 (281 or 282 per quintile). There were no significant differences between the quintiles (Tukey-Kramer multiple-comparisons test).

 

View larger version (10K):
FIGURE 2. . Mean (± SEM) scores (0–100) on the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination (3MSE) for subjects in 3 age groups, showing the relation between global cognitive function score and age. The scores were adjusted for folate, vitamin B-12, homocysteine, creatinine, sex, education, and acculturation of subjects. n = 693, 565, and 148 in the 60–69-y-old, 70–79-y-old, and =" BORDER="0"> 80-y-old groups, respectively. Bars with different letters are significantly different, P < 0.001 (Tukey-Kramer multiple-comparisons test).

 

View larger version (11K):
FIGURE 3. . Mean (± SEM) scores (0–100) on the Modified Mini-Mental State Examination (3MSE) for subjects in 4 education groups, showing the relation between global cognitive function score and education. The scores were adjusted for folate, vitamin B-12, homocysteine, creatinine, age, sex, and acculturation of subjects. n = 543, 282, 334, and 247 in the 0–5-y, 6–8-y, 9–12-y, and > 12-y groups, respectively. Bars with different letters are significantly different, P 0.01 (Tukey-Kramer multiple-comparisons test).

 

DISCUSSION  
The primary observation of this study is that there are statistically significant associations between homocysteine and several quantifiable measures of cognitive function in a community-dwelling, elderly, Latino population. Notably, these associations are independent of folate and vitamin B-12 status and of kidney function, as reflected in the serum creatinine concentrations. The associations are modest, however: homocysteine predicted 5% of the variance in cognitive function scores in this study sample. In contrast, demographic variables, particularly age and education, were found to be much stronger determinants of cognitive function scores.

These findings are generally consistent with previous assessments of the relations between homocysteine and cognitive function in various at-risk populations, including patients with dementia and other psychogeriatric conditions (12–15), and community-dwelling older adults (16–21). There are, however, some discrepancies. Morris et al (18) recently examined the relation between homocysteine and cognitive function in a subset of the third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES III). Elderly participants in that study (aged =" BORDER="0"> 60 y) with hyperhomocysteinemia (defined as homocysteine concentration > 13.7 µmol/L) were more likely to score poorly on tests of word recall and story idea recall than were their counterparts without hyperhomocysteinemia. In the present study, however, no significant association was observed between homocysteine concentrations and delayed recall. The reason for this difference from the NHANES III data is unclear, but it may be related to significant demographic differences between the studies, including the ethnicity, education levels, and range of cognitive abilities of the study subjects. In another study with discrepant results, carried out in Rotterdam, Netherlands, Kalmijn et al (20) reported that homocysteine did not correlate with Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) scores in a cross-sectional analysis of 702 community-dwelling older adults (aged =" BORDER="0"> 55 y), nor did homocysteine correlate with a decline in MMSE score over time. The present study used a modified version of the MMSE, the 3MSE, which is considered to have a somewhat better capacity for discrimination than does the MMSE (24). The use of this modified test may explain why a modest association between homocysteine and global cognitive function was observed in the present study, but not in the Rotterdam study. Another possible contributor to this difference may be the larger sample size of the SALSA cohort.

A potential confounding factor that must be considered in the present study is folic acid fortification. The participants in SALSA were recruited after the institution of folic acid fortification in the United States (fully implemented by January 1998). The efficacy of this program in lowering the prevalence of folate deficiency was previously documented in a report on the Framingham Study (28) and is reflected by the very low prevalence of folate deficiency in the SALSA sample (< 1% defined as RBC folate 160 ng/mL). Folic acid is the most important modifiable determinant of plasma homocysteine concentrations (29), and, consequently, folic acid fortification may have attenuated the relation between homocysteine and cognitive function. This could have occurred as a consequence of reduced homocysteine concentrations or through a homocysteine-independent mechanism. Alternatively, folic acid fortification may have obscured the hyperhomocysteinemia that existed in some study participants before fortification. Sheshadri et al (10) showed that incident dementia over an 8-y period was higher in individuals with the highest baseline concentrations of homocysteine. It is possible that post-folic acid–fortification homocysteine concentrations are less predictive of cognitive function scores than are prefortification homocysteine concentrations. This hypothesis cannot be tested in the SALSA population because prefortification blood samples are not available. However, it should be noted that, despite a low prevalence of folate deficiency, 17% of the SALSA participants had hyperhomocysteinemia (defined as total plasma homocysteine =" BORDER="0"> 13 µmol/L). On the basis of the findings of Seshadri et al (10), it may be predicted that these subjects with current hyperhomocysteinemia are at increased risk of accelerated cognitive decline and dementia over the next decade.

This raises the important question of whether interventions designed to lower plasma homocysteine concentrations will prevent cognitive decline in older adults. The most effective means of lowering homocysteine concentrations is B-vitamin supplementation, and intervention trials are currently underway to determine whether B vitamins reduce the risk of vascular disease (30). It will be interesting to see if the risk of dementia is also reduced in these studies. However, the modest association between homocysteine concentration and cognitive function observed in the present study leads to the conclusion that the effect of B vitamins may be limited.


ACKNOWLEDGMENTS  
We thank Teresa Ortiz and the staff of the Sacramento Area Latino Study on Aging for subject recruitment, phlebotomy, data collection, and data management. We thank Rebecca Cotterman, Jennifer Linfor, and the University of California, Davis, Clinical Laboratory for blood sample processing and biochemical assessments.

JWM participated in the concept and design of the study, supervised blood processing and biochemical analyses, participated in the statistical analysis and interpretation of the data, and was responsible for drafting the article. RG participated in the concept and design of the study, participated in the interpretation of the data, and provided input on the final draft of the article. MIR participated in the statistical analysis and interpretation of the data and provided input on the final draft of the article. LHA (Principal Investigator of US Department of Agriculture grant 5456) participated in the concept and design of the study, participated in the interpretation of the data, and provided input on the final draft of the article. DMM was responsible for development of several of the cognitive function tests used in the study, participated in the statistical analysis and interpretation of the data, and provided input on the final draft of the article. WJJ (Principal Investigator of NIH grant R01 AG10129) participated in the concept and design of the study, participated in the interpretation of the data, and provided input on the final draft of the article. MNH (Principal Investigator of the Sacramento Area Latino Study on Aging, NIH grant R01 AG12975) participated in the concept and design of the study; was responsible for the recruitment of study subjects, the acquisition of blood samples, data collection, and data management; participated in the statistical analysis and interpretation of the data; and provided input on the final draft of the article. None of the authors had a personal or financial conflict of interest with respect to this study.


REFERENCES  

  1. Slooter AJ, Tang MX, van Duijn CM, et al. Apolipoprotein E epsilon 4 and the risk of dementia with stroke: a population based investigation. JAMA 1997;277:818–21.
  2. Breteler MM, Claus JJ. Cardiovascular disease and distribution of cognitive function in elderly people: the Rotterdam Study. BMJ 1994;308:1604–8.
  3. Phillips NA, Mate-Kole C. Cognitive deficits in peripheral vascular disease: a comparison of mild stroke patients and normal control subjects. Stroke 1997;28:777–84.
  4. Launer LJ, Masaki K, Petrovitch H, Foley D, Havlik RJ. The association between midlife blood pressure levels and late-life cognitive function: the Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. JAMA 1995;274:1846–51.
  5. Ott A, Stolk RP, Hofman A, van Harskamp F, Grobbee DE, Breteler MM. Association of diabetes mellitus and dementia: the Rotterdam Study. Diabetologia 1996;39:1392–7.
  6. Refsum H, Ueland PM, Nygard O, Vollset SE. Homocysteine and cardiovascular disease. Annu Rev Med 1998;49:31–62.
  7. Clarke R, Smith AD, Jobst KA, Refsum H, Sutton L, Ueland PM. Folate, vitamin B12, and serum total homocysteine levels in confirmed Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 1998;55:1449–55.
  8. Nilsson K, Gustafson L, Faldt R, et al. Hyperhomocysteinemia—a common finding in a psychogeriatric population. Eur J Clin Invest 1996;26:853–9.
  9. Joosten E, Lesaffre E, Riezler R, et al. Is metabolic evidence for vitamin-B12 and folate deficiency more frequent in elderly patients with Alzheimer’s disease? J Gerontol 1997;52:76–9.
  10. Seshadri S, Beiser A, Selhub J, et al. Plasma homocysteine as a risk factor for dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. N Engl J Med 2002;346:476–83.
  11. Miller JW, Green R, Mungas DM, Reed BR, Jagust WJ. Homocysteine, vitamin B6, and vascular disease in AD patients. Neurology 2002;58:1471–5.
  12. Bell IR, Edman JS, Selhub J, et al. Plasma homocysteine in vascular disease and in nonvascular dementia of depressed elderly people. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1992;86:386–90.
  13. McCaddon A, Davies G, Hudson P, Tandy S, Cattell H. Total serum homocysteine in senile dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 1998;13:235–9.
  14. Lehmann M, Gottfries CG, Regland B. Identification of cognitive impairment in the elderly: homocysteine is an early marker. Dementia Geriatr Cogn Disord 1999;10:12–20.
  15. Nilsson K, Gustafson L, Hultberg B. The plasma homocysteine concentration is better than that of serum methylmalonic acid as a marker for sociopsychological performance in a psychogeriatric population. Clin Chem 2000;46:691–6.
  16. Riggs KM, Spiro A, Tucker K, Rush D. Relations of vitamin B-12, vitamin B-6, folate, and homocysteine to cognitive performance in the Normative Aging Study. Am J Clin Nutr 1996;63:306–14.
  17. Budge M, Johnston C, Hogervorst E, et al. Plasma total homocysteine and cognitive performance in a volunteer elderly population. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2000;903:407–10.
  18. Morris MS, Jacques PF, Rosenberg IH, Selhub J. Hyperhomocysteinemia associated with poor recall in the third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. Am J Clin Nutr 2001;73:927–33.
  19. Duthie SJ, Whalley LJ, Collins AR, Leaper S, Berger K, Deary IJ. Homocysteine, B vitamin status, and cognitive function in the elderly. Am J Clin Nutr 2002;75:908–13.
  20. Kalmijn S, Launer LJ, Lindemans J, Bots ML, Hofman A, Breteler MM. Total homocysteine and cognitive decline in a community-based sample of elderly subjects: the Rotterdam Study. Am J Epidemiol 1999;150:283–9.
  21. Ravaglia G, Forti P, Maioli F, et al. Blood homocysteine and vitamin B levels are not associated with cognitive skills in healthy normally ageing subjects. J Nutr Health Aging 2000;4:218–22.
  22. Wu CC, Mungas D, Petkov CI, et al. Brain structure and cognition in a community sample of elderly Latinos. Neurology 2002;59:383–91.
  23. Gilfix BM, Blank DW, Rosenblatt DS. Novel reductant for determination of total plasma homocysteine. Clin Chem 1997;43:687–8.
  24. Teng EL, Chui HC. The Modified Mini-Mental State (3MS) examination. J Clin Psychiatry 1987;48:314–8.
  25. Mungas DM, Reed BR, Marshall SC, Gonzalez HM. Development of psychometrically matched English and Spanish language neuropsychological tests for older persons. Neuropsychology 2000;14:1–15.
  26. Cuellar I, Harris LC, Jasso R. An acculturation scale for Mexican American normal and clinical populations. Hispanic J Behav Sci 1980;2:199–217.
  27. Gonzalez HM, Haan MN, Hinton L. Acculturation and the prevalence of depression in older Mexican Americans: baseline results of the Sacramento Area Latino Study on Aging. J Am Geriatr Soc 2001:49:948–53.
  28. Jacques PF, Selhub J, Bostom AG, Wilson PW, Rosenberg IH. The effect of folic acid fortification on plasma folate and total homocysteine concentrations. N Engl J Med 1999;340:1449–54.
  29. Selhub J, Jacques PF, Wilson PW, et al. Vitamin status and intake as primary determinants of homocysteinemia in an elderly population. JAMA 1993;270:2693–8.
  30. Clarke R. An overview of the homocysteine lowering clinical trials. In: Robinson K, ed. Homocysteine and vascular disease. Norwell, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers, 2000:413–29.
Received for publication October 23, 2002. Accepted for publication February 18, 2003.


Related articles in AJCN:

Vitamins, homocysteine, and cognition
Sally P Stabler
AJCN 2003 78: 359-360. [Full Text]  

作者: Joshua W Miller
医学百科App—中西医基础知识学习工具
  • 相关内容
  • 近期更新
  • 热文榜
  • 医学百科App—健康测试工具