Literature
首页医源资料库在线期刊美国临床营养学杂志2004年80卷第4期

Alcohol intake in relation to brain magnetic resonance imaging findings in older persons without dementia

来源:《美国临床营养学杂志》
摘要:However,alowconcentrationofalcoholmayalsohavedirecteffectsonthehippocampus,abrainstructurehighlyaffectedbyAlzheimerdisease。Objective:Weinvestigatedalcoholintakeinrelationtobrainmagneticresonanceimaging(MRI)findingsofpresumedvascularorigin(ie,whitematter......

点击显示 收起

Tom den Heijer, Sarah E Vermeer, Ewoud J van Dijk, Niels D Prins, Peter J Koudstaal, Cornelia M van Duijn, Albert Hofman and Monique MB Breteler

1 From the Departments of Epidemiology & Biostatistics (TdH, SEV, EjvD, NDP, CMvD, AH, and MMMB) and of Neurology (TdH, SEV, EjvD, NDP, and PJK), Erasmus Medical Center, Rotterdam, Netherlands.

2 Supported by the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO grant 904-61-096), the Netherlands Heart Foundation (grant 97.152), and the Health Research and Development Council (ZON).

3 Address reprint requests to MMB Breteler, Department of Epidemiology & Biostatistics, Erasmus Medical Center, PO Box 1738, 3000 DR Rotterdam, Netherlands. E-mail: m.breteler{at}erasmusmc.nl.


ABSTRACT  
Background: Consumers of light-to-moderate amounts of alcohol have a lower risk of dementia and, possibly, Alzheimer disease than do abstainers. Because vascular disease may contribute to symptoms of Alzheimer disease, reduction of cerebrovascular disease in consumers of light amounts of alcohol could account for that observation. However, a low concentration of alcohol may also have direct effects on the hippocampus, a brain structure highly affected by Alzheimer disease.

Objective: We investigated alcohol intake in relation to brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) findings of presumed vascular origin (ie, white matter lesions and infarcts) and findings more specifically found in early Alzheimer disease (ie, hippocampal and amygdalar atrophy).

Design: In a population-based sample of 1074 older persons without dementia (aged 60–90 y), we made brain MRIs from which we rated white matter lesions and brain infarcts. In a subset of 509 people, hippocampal and amygdalar volumes on MRI were measured. Alcohol intake was assessed by using a structured questionnaire. We categorized alcohol intake as lifetime abstention and very light (<1 drink/wk), light (1 drink/wk to <1 drink/d), moderate (1 drink/d to <4 drinks/d), and heavy (4 drinks/d) intakes.

Results: Persons whose alcohol consumption was light to moderate had less severe white matter lesions and brain infarcts on MRI than did abstainers or heavy drinkers. Abstainers and very light drinkers had smaller hippocampal and amygdalar volumes on MRI than did light-to-moderate drinkers, but only if the former carried an apolipoprotein (APOE) 4 allele.

Conclusion: Light-to-moderate alcohol intake is associated with a lower prevalence of vascular brain findings and, in APOE 4 carriers, hippocampal and amygdalar atrophy on MRI.

Key Words: Alcohol • Alzheimer disease • brain • hippocampus • amygdala • magnetic resonance imaging


INTRODUCTION  
Several prospective, population-based studies have shown that persons with light-to-moderate consumption of alcohol have a lower risk of dementia than do persons who abstain or have heavy alcohol use (1–4). Most studies find a relation with vascular dementia (2, 4, 5) that is consistent with the known beneficial effects of light-to-moderate alcohol consumption on vascular risk profile (6) and risk of stroke (7). Vascular disease likely contributes to the clinical syndrome in a large proportion of elderly Alzheimer patients (8, 9). A plausible explanation for the associations that are seen between alcohol consumption and overall dementia or Alzheimer disease (3, 4, 10) is that they have to do with an effect on vascular disease. Alternatively, alcohol in low amounts could have stimulatory effects on the release of acetylcholine in the hippocampus (11), and other nonvascular mechanisms might play a role (12). A nonvascular relation between alcohol intake and Alzheimer disease was indeed suggested by our observation in the Rotterdam Study that alcohol intake seemed to be associated with Alzheimer disease only in carriers of the apolipoprotein (APOE) 4 allele (2), which is the risk allele for Alzheimer disease (13).

To further investigate what could underlie the relation between alcohol intake and risk of dementia, we decided to focus on the relation between alcohol intake and structural brain findings on magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) of older persons without dementia. We hypothesized that, if the effect of alcohol on dementia risk purely had to do with an effect on vascular disease, we would see a relation between alcohol intake and markers of cerebrovascular disease [eg, brain infarcts and white matter lesions (9, 14–16)], but not between alcohol intake and putative presymptomatic MRI markers of Alzheimer disease [eg, hippocampal and amygdalar atrophy (17–22)]. We investigated this hypothesis in the population-based Rotterdam Scan Study.


SUBJECTS AND METHODS  
Study sample
This study was based on data collected in the Rotterdam Scan Study, a population-based cohort study designed to investigate the determinants of age-related brain changes as seen on MRI and their consequences (23). The study protocol has been described in detail elsewhere (24). The study’s baseline examinations were conducted in1995 and 1996. At that time, we made a random selection in strata of age (5 y) and sex of 1904 persons aged 60–90 y originating from 2 population cohort studies. We excluded from this sample persons with dementia, according to the protocol of the Rotterdam Study (25) for detecting dementia. Briefly, this stepwise protocol started with a cognitive screen with the Mini-Mental State Examination and Geriatric Mental Schedule organic part A. Persons who were positive on screening underwent further neuropsychological testing and, if indicated, were examined by a neurologist. After the exclusion of persons with dementia or contraindications to MRI [eg, claustrophobia or the presence of implanted metal clips (for cerebral aneurysms) or a pacemaker], 1717 persons were eligible, of whom 1077 participated and gave written informed consent (participation rate: 63%; mean age: 72.2 y; proportion of women: 52%). All 1077 persons underwent MRI sequences for assessment of white matter lesions and brain infarcts. In a subset of 563 of these 1077, the MRI was done with the use of a unit (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) that allowed us to include a 3-dimensional (3-D) MRI sequence needed for the assessment of hippocampal and amygdalar volumes. The Medical Ethics Committee of Erasmus Medical Center approved the study protocol.

Alcohol intake
A physician used a structured questionnaire to evaluate the alcohol intake of each participant at baseline. Alcohol intake was coded as lifetime abstention, former drinking, and current drinking (defined as having consumed alcohol during the past 12 mo). Current and former drinkers were asked about the amount of their alcohol consumption. The amount assessed with this questionnaire correlated well with the information obtained from a food-frequency questionnaire administered in part of the sample 5 y earlier (Pearson’s r = 0.72, P < 0.01; 26). We categorized current alcohol drinkers as very light (<1 drink/wk), light (1 drink/wk to <1 drink/d), moderate (1 drink/d to <4 drinks/d), and heavy (4 drinks/d) drinkers, as in our previous report on dementia (2). One alcoholic drink (a glass of beer, wine, fortified wine, or spirits) was considered to contain 13 g alcohol. Binge drinking was defined as drinking 6 glasses in 1 d during the 6 mo before the interview. For 3 participants, information on alcohol intake was missing.

MRI acquisition
All 1077 participants underwent axial T1-, T2-, and proton density-weighted brain MRI scanning with a 1.5-Tesla unit (Philips, Best, Netherlands, or Siemens) at baseline (24). Only the Siemens MRI unit, in which 563 participants had their MRI scan, allowed us also to include a custom-made 3-D MRI sequence (half-Fourier Acquisition Single-Shot Turbo-Spin Echo; 27) needed for volumetric assessment of the hippocampus and amygdala. Fifty-two of the 563 participants experienced claustrophobia during the MRI scan, and 2 others provided no information on alcohol intake, which left 509 participants with information on alcohol intake and hippocampal and amygdalar volumes.

Cerebrovascular disease on MRI
MRI measurements were performed by investigators who were blinded to clinical information of the participants (24, 28). White matter lesions were considered present if visible as hyperintense on proton-density–and T2-weighted images, without prominent hypointensity on T1-weighted scans. Periventricular white matter lesions were scored semiquantitatively from 0 to 9, and subcortical white matter lesions were counted in different size categories to approximate a total lesion volume (in mL; 24). We defined infarcts as focal hyperintensities on T2-weighted images. Infarcts in the white matter also were required to have corresponding hypointensities on T1-weighted images to distinguish them from white matter lesions. Infarcts on MRI were classified as silent or symptomatic (28).

Hippocampal and amygdalar volumes on MRI
We constructed a series of coronal brain slices (contiguous 1.5-mm slice thickness) from the 3-D MRI, which were aligned to be perpendicular to the long axis of the hippocampus. We manually traced the boundaries of the hippocampus and amygdala on both sides on each slice by using a mouse-driven cursor (27). The summed surface areas were multiplied by slice thickness to yield estimates of the hippocampal and amygdalar volumes (mL). The left- and right-sided volumes were summed to yield the total hippocampal and amygdalar volumes. As a proxy for head size, we measured on the middle sagittal MRI slice the intracranial cross-sectional area (27). We corrected for head size differences among subjects by dividing the uncorrected volumes by the subject’s calculated head size area and subsequently multiplying this ratio by the average head size area (separately for men and women; 29, 30).

Covariates
The following covariates were assessed at baseline by interview and physical examination: pack-years of cigarette smoking, educational level, body mass index (BMI; in kg/m2), diabetes mellitus, and hypertension (28). APOE genotype testing was performed (31) and available for 969 participants in the total sample and 436 participants in the subset with hippocampal and amygdalar volume assessment; testing was not performed in the rest of the subjects, mainly because no blood from them was available. Participants were classified as carriers or noncarriers of an APOE 4 allele; those with genotype APOE 24 (n = 22 in the total sample and 9 in the subsample) were excluded from analyses considering APOE genotype.

Statistical analysis
The analyses of alcohol intake and white matter lesions and infarcts were based on 1074 persons, and the analyses of alcohol intake and hippocampal and amygdalar volumes were based on the subset of 509 persons. We used multivariable linear regression to quantify the association between alcohol-intake categories and white matter lesions and hippocampal and amygdalar volumes. With logistic regression, we calculated the adjusted odds ratio (95% CI) of brain infarcts across alcohol-intake categories by using abstainers as the reference category. For tests of linear trend, we treated the categories of alcohol intake as the continuous variable. For tests of quadratic trend, we squared the linear trend variable. We included age, sex, and pack-years of cigarette smoking as covariates. In addition, we adjusted for educational level, BMI, diabetes, and hypertension. We evaluated whether the effects of alcohol differed by sex or APOE genotype (carrier or noncarrier of the APOE 4 allele) by performing stratified analyses and including interaction terms in the model. Statistical analyses were performed by using SPSS statistical software for WINDOWS (version 11.5; SPSS Inc, Chicago).


RESULTS  
There were no differences in baseline characteristics between the total sample and the subsample in which hippocampal and amygdalar volumes on MRI were evaluated (Table 1). Characteristics according to alcohol intake categories are given for the total sample in Table 2. Former drinkers (n = 86 in the total sample and n = 42 in the subsample) had hypertension and diabetes mellitus more frequently than did the current drinkers, which supports our presumption that former drinkers might have stopped drinking because of illness. Therefore, former drinkers were excluded from analyses of alcohol intake and brain MRI findings.


View this table:
TABLE 1. Characteristics of the total study sample and of the subsample with assessment of hippocampal and amygdalar volumes on 3-dimensional (3-D) magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)

 
Light alcohol drinkers had the smallest amount of periventricular white matter lesions on MRI [adjusted difference from abstainers: 0.69 (95% CI: 0.25, 1.13; P = 0.002); adjusted difference from heavy drinkers: 0.79 (95% CI: 0.17, 1.41; P = 0.01)], as shown in Table 3. The light alcohol drinkers also tended to have fewer subcortical white matter lesions than did the heavy drinkers, but the difference did not reach significance [adjusted difference from abstainers: 0.57 (95% CI: –0.05, 1.20; P = 0.07); adjusted difference from heavy drinkers: 0.35 (95% CI: –0.54, 1.24; P = 0.44)]. Brain infarcts on MRI tended to be found less frequently in very light and light drinkers, but none of the odds ratios reached statistical significance (Table 3). When we excluded persons with symptomatic infarcts on MRI (n = 42)—because they could have changed their alcohol intake after the stroke—the results remained similar. The associations did not change after additional adjustments for educational level, BMI, diabetes, and hypertension, and they were similar in men and women and in APOE 4 carriers and noncarriers.


View this table:
TABLE 3. Vascular brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) findings according to alcohol intake in the total sample

 
Overall, there was no association between alcohol intake and hippocampal or amygdalar volumes on MRI, as shown in Table 4. However, the effect of alcohol intake on volumes differed by APOE genotype (P for interaction = 0.02 for hippocampal volume and = 0.07 for amygdalar volume). In carriers of the 4 allele, alcohol intake was positively associated with hippocampal and amygdalar volumes, whereas, in 4 noncarriers, there was no association (Figure 1). The associations between alcohol intake and hippocampal or amygdalar volumes did not change after additional adjustment for educational level, BMI, diabetes, and hypertension, and they were similar in men and women.


View larger version (20K):
FIGURE 1.. Hippocampal and amygdalar volumes on magnetic resonance imaging according to alcohol intake by strata of APOE genotype (4 noncarriers: n = 288, ; 4 carriers: n = 101, ). Volumes were adjusted for age, sex, and pack-years of cigarette smoking and normalized to average head size. Error bars indicate SEM. For the analyses of hippocampal volumes, the P value for linear trend in APOE 4 noncarriers was 0.15 and that in 4 carriers was 0.009. For the analyses of amygdalar volumes, the P value for linear trend in APOE 4 noncarriers was 0.84 and that in 4 carriers was 0.02. The P value of the interaction term between APOE genotype and alcohol intake was 0.02 for hippocampal volume and 0.07 for amygdalar volume.

 
In the total sample, there were 111 persons who fulfilled the criteria for binge drinking (6 alcoholic drinks in1 d). Of this group, 56 were also in the subset with hippocampal and amygdalar data, and excluding them did not change the results.


DISCUSSION  
In this population-based study in older persons, we found that drinkers of light-to-moderate amounts of alcohol had fewer cerebral white matter lesions and brain infarcts on MRI than did abstainers and heavy drinkers. In APOE 4 carriers, but not in 4 noncarriers, we found a positive association between alcohol intake and hippocampal and amygdalar volumes on MRI.

Some aspects of the methods used in this study should be discussed. First, there could be error in the MRI measures, particularly in the semiquantitative assessments. However, given that MRI assessments were done with the investigators blinded to the subjects’ alcohol intake, this is random error that only leads to weaker associations between alcohol and MRI measures. Second, we relied on self-reported alcohol intake, which may have led to underreporting or overreporting. Although we may have misclassified absolute amounts of alcohol intake, the ranking of subjects according to their alcohol intake most likely was adequate (32). If persons who actually drink heavily underreported their alcohol consumption, we will have underestimated the difference in risk between those who drink moderately and abstainers. Third, we assessed average alcohol intake over a 1-y period before MRI examinations, but it might be better to have information on the average alcohol intake over the lifetime. Moreover, we lacked detailed information on drinking patterns, which may be important in estimating the effects of alcohol. Fourth, we had no information on the type of alcoholic drink consumed. Previous studies showed that beer, spirits, and wine—consumed in moderation—are equally associated with a reduced risk of dementia (2, 4), although the Copenhagen City Heart Study found the association only for moderate wine consumption (10). Drinking patterns in the older Dutch population studied here were reported before (2); they show that, among alcohol consumers, 35% drink wine, 35% drink spirits, 30% drink fortified wine, and 20% drink beer. Finally, given that our study is observational and that alcohol intake may be associated with several unmeasured confounders, we cannot infer causality of the associations.

Light-to-moderate alcohol drinking is associated with a lower risk of cognitive impairment and dementia than is lifetime abstention (1–4, 10, 33, 34). In line with the beneficial effects of alcohol on the lipid profile, hemostatic factors (6), and atherosclerosis (35), most investigators found a reduced risk of vascular dementia in consumers of light-to-moderate amounts of alcohol (2, 4, 5). We found that, consistent with these vascular effects of alcohol and with observations in the Cardiovascular Health Study (36), consumers of light-to-moderate amounts of alcohol had fewer cerebral white matter lesions—in particular, those in the periventricular region—and infarcts on MRI. Different pathophysiologic events may lead to the development of either periventricular or subcortical white matter lesions (14). The periventricular white matter, in particular, is vulnerable to ischemia, and severe periventricular white matter lesions are related to carotid atherosclerosis (37). The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities study of middle-aged subjects did not show an effect of alcohol intake on cerebrovascular disease on MRI, but those investigators combined moderate and heavy alcohol intake into one category, which may have diluted any beneficial effect (38). Light-to-moderate alcohol drinking has also been associated with a reduced risk of Alzheimer disease in several (1, 3, 4, 10, 39), but not all (40, 41) studies. Besides reducing cerebrovascular disease that contributes to the development of symptoms of Alzheimer disease (8, 9, 25), alcohol in moderate amounts may also increase the release of acetylcholine of the hippocampus (11) and induce the release of potentially beneficial prostaglandins (12). In traditional Alzheimer disease, amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tangles accumulate in the brain, with a high predilection for the medial temporal lobe (42). This may lead to neuronal loss and atrophy in vivo that are detectable on MRI even in persons who have not yet developed clinical symptoms of dementia (17–22). APOE 4 carriers are at increased risk of Alzheimer disease-related neurologic disorders (43), atrophy of structures in the medial temporal lobe (44), and clinical Alzheimer disease (13). Observations in the Rotterdam Study suggested that light-to-moderate alcohol consumption is associated with a reduced risk of Alzheimer disease only in APOE 4 carriers (2). Carmelli et al (45) also reported a stronger association of light drinking and better cognitive performance in APOE 4 carriers than in noncarriers. In the current study, we found that APOE 4 carriers who were consumers of light-to-moderate amounts of alcohol had less hippocampal and amygdalar atrophy than did abstainers. Although not all persons with hippocampal or amygdalar atrophy will develop Alzheimer disease, such persons are at increased risk of clinical symptoms of Alzheimer disease (18), and, thus, our findings support the hypothesis that there could be a reduced risk of Alzheimer disease for consumers of light-to-moderate amounts of alcohol who have the APOE 4 allele. It is currently unclear which biological mechanisms could be involved in the associations between alcohol and Alzheimer disease. We speculate that a putative antioxidative effect of alcohol (46) might suppress the highly sensitive peroxidation of APOE 4 (47) and thereby reduce amyloid plaque formation in the medial temporal lobe (48, 49) or neurotoxicity of the ß-amyloid (50). Unlike the current study, the Cardiovascular Health Study found a lower risk of dementia in alcohol consumers to be more consistent among persons without the APOE 4 allele (4). It is possible that the youth of the population studied in the Rotterdam Study compared with the age of the Cardiovascular Health Study population led to these discrepancies, because the APOE 4 allele is a stronger risk factor for Alzheimer disease at younger ages (13). The Epidemiology of Vascular Aging study also found a reduced risk of cognitive deterioration with alcohol intake in 4 noncarriers, but, in that study, the APOE 4 allele itself was not a risk factor for cognitive decline (51).

In conclusion, our findings suggest that light-to-moderate alcohol intake is associated with brain MRI findings that are related to cerebrovascular disease and—in APOE 4 carriers—with hippocampal and amygdalar atrophy on MRI. These findings are in line with recent observations that light-to-moderate alcohol consumption is associated with a reduced risk of dementia.


View this table:
TABLE 2. Characteristics according to current alcohol intake in the total sample1

 

View this table:
TABLE 4. Hippocampal and amygdalar volume on magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) according to alcohol intake in the subsample1

 

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS  
We thank JC de Groot, F-E de Leeuw, F Hoebeek, and E Achten for their contribution to data collection. We thank the general practitioners and pharmacists in Ommoord for their contributions to the Rotterdam Study.

TdH, SEV, EJvD, NDP, PJK, CMvD, AH, and MMBB were responsible for the study concept and design; (TdH, SEV, EJvD, NDP, PJK, CMvD, AH, and MMBB were responsible for the analysis and interpretation of data; TdH and MMBB were responsible for drafting the manuscript; and (TdH, SEV, EJvD, NDP, PJK, CMvD, AH, and MMBB were responsible for critically revising the manuscript. None of the authors had conflicts of interest that relate to the manuscript.


REFERENCES  

  1. Orgogozo JM, Dartigues JF, Lafont S, et al. Wine consumption and dementia in the elderly: a prospective community study in the Bordeaux area. Rev Neurol (Paris) 1997;153:185–92.
  2. Ruitenberg A, van Swieten JC, Witteman JCM, et al. Alcohol consumption and risk of dementia: the Rotterdam Study. Lancet 2002;359:281–6.
  3. Huang W, Qiu C, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L. Alcohol consumption and incidence of dementia in a community sample aged 75 years and older. J Clin Epidemiol 2002;55:959–64.
  4. Mukamal KJ, Kuller LH, Fitzpatrick AL, Longstreth WT Jr, Mittleman MA, Siscovick DS. Prospective study of alcohol consumption and risk of dementia in older adults. JAMA 2003;289:1405–13.
  5. Hébert R, Lindsay J, Verreault R, Rockwood K, Hill G, Dubois MF. Vascular dementia: incidence and risk factors in the Canadian study of health and aging. Stroke 2000;31:1487–93.
  6. Rimm EB, Williams P, Fosher K, Criqui M, Stampfer MJ. Moderate alcohol intake and lower risk of coronary heart disease: meta-analysis of effects on lipids and haemostatic factors. BMJ 1999;319:1523–8.
  7. Reynolds K, Lewis B, Nolen JD, Kinney GL, Sathya B, He J. Alcohol consumption and risk of stroke: a meta-analysis. JAMA 2003;289:579–88.
  8. de la Torre JC. Alzheimer disease as a vascular disorder: nosological evidence. Stroke 2002;33:1152–62.
  9. Kalaria RN. The role of cerebral ischemia in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 2000;21:321–30.
  10. Truelsen T, Thudium D, Grønbæk M. Amount and type of alcohol and risk of dementia: the Copenhagen City Heart Study. Neurology 2002;59:1313–9.
  11. Henn C, Loffelholz K, Klein J. Stimulatory and inhibitory effects of ethanol on hippocampal acetylcholine release. Naunyn Schmiedebergs Arch Pharmacol 1998;357:640–7.
  12. Das UN. Alcohol consumption and risk of dementia. Lancet 2002;360:490 (letter).
  13. Farrer LA, Cupples LA, Haines JL, et al. Effects of age, sex, and ethnicity on the association between apolipoprotein E genotype and Alzheimer disease. A meta-analysis. APOE and Alzheimer Disease Meta-Analysis Consortium. JAMA 1997;278:1349–56.
  14. Pantoni L, Garcia JH. Pathogenesis of leukoaraiosis: a review. Stroke 1997;28:652–9.
  15. Wong TY, Klein R, Sharrett AR, et al. Cerebral white matter lesions, retinopathy, and incident clinical stroke. JAMA 2002;288:67–74.
  16. Barber R, Scheltens P, Gholkar A, et al. White matter lesions on magnetic resonance imaging in dementia with lewy bodies, Alzheimer’s disease, vascular dementia, and normal aging. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1999;67:66–72.
  17. Bobinski M, de Leon MJ, Wegiel J, et al. The histological validation of post mortem magnetic resonance imaging-determined hippocampal volume in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroscience 2000;95:721–5.
  18. Jack CRJ, Petersen RC, Xu YC, et al. Prediction of AD with MRI-based hippocampal volume in mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 1999;52:1397–403.
  19. Scheltens P, Fox N, Barkhof F, De Carli C. Structural magnetic resonance imaging in the practical assessment of dementia: beyond exclusion. Lancet Neurol 2002;1:13–21.
  20. Mizuno K, Wakai M, Takeda A, Sobue G. Medial temporal atrophy and memory impairment in early stage of Alzheimer’s disease: an MRI volumetric and memory assessment study. J Neurol Sci 2000;173:18–24.
  21. Killiany RJ, Gomez-Isla T, Moss M, et al. Use of structural magnetic resonance imaging to predict who will get Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol 2000;47:430–9.
  22. Convit A, de Leon MJ, Tarshish C, et al. Hippocampal volume losses in minimally impaired elderly. Lancet 1995;345:266 (letter).
  23. Breteler MMB. Vascular involvement in cognitive decline and dementia. Epidemiologic evidence from the Rotterdam Study and the Rotterdam Scan Study. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2000;903:457–65.
  24. de Groot JC, de Leeuw FE, Oudkerk M, et al. Cerebral white matter lesions and cognitive function: the Rotterdam Scan Study. Ann Neurol 2000;47:145–51.
  25. Vermeer SE, Prins ND, den Heijer T, Hofman A, Koudstaal PJ, Breteler MMB. Silent brain infarcts and the risk of dementia and cognitive decline. N Engl J Med 2003;348:1215–22.
  26. Klipstein-Grobusch K, den Breeijen JH, Goldbohm RA, et al. Dietary assessment in the elderly: validation of a semiquantitative food frequency questionnaire. Eur J Clin Nutr 1998;52:588–96.
  27. Hackert VH, den Heijer T, Oudkerk M, Koudstaal PJ, Hofman A, Breteler MMB. Hippocampal head size associated with verbal memory performance in nondemented elderly. Neuroimage 2002;17:1365–72.
  28. Vermeer SE, Koudstaal PJ, Oudkerk M, Hofman A, Breteler MMB. Prevalence and risk factors of silent brain infarcts in the population-based Rotterdam Scan Study. Stroke 2002;33:21–5.
  29. Callen DJ, Black SE, Gao F, Caldwell CB, Szalai JP. Beyond the hippocampus: MRI volumetry confirms widespread limbic atrophy in AD. Neurology 2001;57:1669–74.
  30. Cuenod CA, Denys A, Michot JL, et al. Amygdala atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease. An in vivo magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch Neurol 1993;50:941–5.
  31. Wenham PR, Price WH, Blandell G. Apolipoprotein E genotyping by one-stage PCR. Lancet 1991;337:1158–9.
  32. Feunekes GI, van ’t Veer P, van Staveren WA, Kok FJ. Alcohol intake assessment: the sober facts. Am J Epidemiol 1999;150:105–12.
  33. Dufouil C, Ducimetiere P, Alperovitch A. Sex differences in the association between alcohol consumption and cognitive performance. EVA Study Group. Epidemiology of Vascular Aging. Am J Epidemiol 1997;146:405–12.
  34. Elias PK, Elias MF, D’Agostino RB, Silbershatz H, Wolf PA. Alcohol consumption and cognitive performance in the Framingham Heart Study. Am J Epidemiol 1999;150:580–9.
  35. Kiechl S, Willeit J, Rungger G, Egger G, Oberhollenzer F, Bonora E. Alcohol consumption and atherosclerosis: what is the relation? Prospective results from the Bruneck Study. Stroke 1998;29:900–7.
  36. Mukamal KJ, Longstreth WT Jr, Mittleman MA, Crum RM, Siscovick DS. Alcohol consumption and subclinical findings on magnetic resonance imaging of the brain in older adults: the cardiovascular health study. Stroke 2001;32:1939–46.
  37. de Leeuw FE, de Groot JC, Bots ML, et al. Carotid atherosclerosis and cerebral white matter lesions in a population based magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurol 2000;247:291–6.
  38. Ding J, Eigenbrodt ML, Mosley TH Jr, et al. Alcohol intake and cerebral abnormalities on magnetic resonance imaging in a community-based population of middle-aged adults: the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) study. Stroke 2004;35:16–21.
  39. Bachman DL, Green RC, Benke KS, Cupples LA, Farrer LA. Comparison of Alzheimer’s disease risk factors in white and African American families. Neurology 2003;60:1372–4.
  40. Tanaka N, Asada T, Kinoshita T, Yamashita F, Uno M. Alcohol consumption and risk of dementia. Lancet 2002;360:491 (letter).
  41. Hebert LE, Scherr PA, Beckett LA, et al. Relation of smoking and alcohol consumption to incident Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Epidemiol 1992;135:347–55.
  42. Braak H, Braak E. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol 1991;82:239–59.
  43. Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Schneider JA, et al. Apolipoprotein E 4 allele, AD pathology, and the clinical expression of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 2003;60:246–52.
  44. den Heijer T, Oudkerk M, Launer LJ, van Duijn CM, Hofman A, Breteler MMB. Hippocampal, amygdalar, and global brain atrophy in different apolipoprotein E genotypes. Neurology 2002;59:746–8.
  45. Carmelli D, Swan GE, Reed T, Schellenberg GD, Christian JC. The effect of apolipoprotein E epsilon4 in the relationships of smoking and drinking to cognitive function. Neuroepidemiology 1999;18:125–33.
  46. Hillbom M. Oxidants, antioxidants, alcohol and stroke. Front Biosci 1999;4:e67–71.
  47. Jolivalt C, Leininger-Muller B, Bertrand P, Herber R, Christen Y, Siest G. Differential oxidation of apolipoprotein E isoforms and interaction with phospholipids. Free Radic Biol Med 2000;28:129–40.
  48. Strittmatter WJ, Weisgraber KH, Huang DY, et al. Binding of human apolipoprotein E to synthetic amyloid beta peptide: isoform-specific effects and implications for late-onset Alzheimer disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1993;90:8098–102.
  49. Pike CJ, Burdick D, Walencewicz AJ, Glabe CG, Cotman CW. Neurodegeneration induced by beta-amyloid peptides in vitro: the role of peptide assembly state. J Neurosci 1993;13:1676–87.
  50. Huang HM, Ou HC, Hsieh SJ. Antioxidants prevent amyloid peptide-induced apoptosis and alteration of calcium homeostasis in cultured cortical neurons. Life Sci 2000;66:1879–92.
  51. Dufouil C, Tzourio C, Brayne C, Berr C, Amouyel P, Alpérovitch A. Influence of apolipoprotein E genotype on the risk of cognitive deterioration in moderate drinkers and smokers. Epidemiology 2000;11:280–4.
Received for publication January 15, 2004. Accepted for publication April 5, 2004.


作者: Tom den Heijer
医学百科App—中西医基础知识学习工具
  • 相关内容
  • 近期更新
  • 热文榜
  • 医学百科App—健康测试工具