Literature
首页医源资料库在线期刊美国临床营养学杂志2005年81卷第3期

Decreases in fasting leptin and insulin concentrations after acute energy restriction and subsequent compensation in food intake

来源:《美国临床营养学杂志》
摘要:nlABSTRACTBackground:Thedecreaseinleptinafterenergyrestrictionisastarvationsignaltothebrain。however,nosoliddataareavailableontheacutedecreaseinfastingleptinconcentrationandsubsequentcaloriccompensation。Objective:Theobjectivewastoassesstheeffectofacute......

点击显示 收起

Monica Mars, Cees de Graaf, Lisette CPGM de Groot and Frans J Kok

1 From the Division of Human Nutrition, Wageningen University, Wageningen, Netherlands

2 Supported by the Netherlands Association for Scientific Research (NWO-MW project no. 980-10-007).

3 Reprints not available. Address correspondence to M Mars, PO Box 8129, NL-6700 EV Wageningen, Netherlands. E-mail: monica.mars{at}wur.nl


ABSTRACT  
Background: The decrease in leptin after energy restriction is a starvation signal to the brain. Several studies have found an association between this decrease and subjective appetite; however, no solid data are available on the acute decrease in fasting leptin concentration and subsequent caloric compensation.

Objective: The objective was to assess the effect of acute decreases in fasting leptin concentrations, induced by energy restriction, on subsequent energy intake compensation. We hypothesized that men with a large decrease in fasting leptin concentrations would have larger ad libitum energy intakes than would men with a small decrease in leptin.

Design: Thirty-four male unrestrained eaters [age: 23 ± 3 y; body mass index (in kg/m2): 22.3 ± 1.6] participated in a semicontrolled intervention study. Fasting serum leptin and insulin concentrations were measured before and 2 d after 62% energy restriction. Energy intake was measured on the 2 following days on which food was provided ad libitum.

Results: During energy restriction, fasting leptin and insulin concentrations decreased by 27.2% (95% CI: –34.4%, –19.9%) and 30.7% (95% CI: –41.0%, –20.4%), respectively. Subjects consumed 143 ± 27% of their estimated energy requirements (18.3 ± 2.9 MJ) on the first day and 124 ± 20% (16.0 ± 2.6 MJ) on the second day of ad libitum intake. No significant correlations were observed between decreases in fasting leptin or insulin concentrations and subsequent ad libitum energy intake; however, decreases in insulin were correlated with an increase in carbohydrate intake (r=–0.49, P < 0.01).

Conclusion: Although fasting leptin concentrations decreased significantly during energy restriction and subjects showed compensatory behavior during subsequent ad libitum food intake, no association was observed between the decrease in fasting leptin concentrations and caloric compensation.

Key Words: Ob protein • insulin • appetite regulation • energy intake • caloric restriction • leptin


INTRODUCTION  
Leptin is a hormone that is produced by adipose tissue and is secreted in the blood (1). Because leptin is produced by fat cells, serum leptin concentrations are positively correlated with the amount of fat mass in the body (2). However, short-term severe energy restriction causes circulating leptin concentrations to decrease to a larger extent than would be expected from the loss of fat mass alone (3–5). In a recent review, Flier (6) advocates that this decrease in leptin concentration serves as an important signal from fat to the brain that the body is starving. Moreover, he suggests that this function is likely to be as important as or more important than is leptin’s function to inform the brain about the amount of fat mass. Because of the low availability of glucose, insulin concentrations decrease during energy restriction. It has been suggested that this decrease in insulin mediates the acute decrease in leptin (5, 7). Moreover, growing evidence suggests that insulin itself is also involved in the central regulation of energy balance via a pathway similar to that for leptin (8, 9).

Although several human intervention studies have shown that decreases in leptin concentrations are associated with increased appetite feelings (10–13), only one study has shown that acute manipulations in energy balance are inversely associated with energy intake compensation (14). However, this study included only 6 obese subjects and the manipulations included both energy restriction and overfeeding. Therefore, the results of that study have to be interpreted carefully. Thus, no solid data are available on the effect of an acute decrease in leptin on energy intake compensation in humans.

For studies measuring energy intake compensation, it is important to select subjects that are lean and do not exhibit restraint eating, ie, a high cognitive awareness of food intake (15) or any other inhibition. Being overweight and a restrained eater may suppress voluntary energy intake after weight loss. Therefore, we performed a study in 34 lean, male, unrestrained, free-living subjects. An acute decrease in fasting leptin and insulin concentrations was induced by 2 d of 62% energy restriction and, subsequently, the ad libitum compensatory energy intake was measured during the 2 d after. We hypothesized that men with a large decrease in leptin would have higher ad libitum energy intakes than would men with a small decrease in leptin. Second, we investigated whether this decrease in leptin was associated with macronutrient composition of the ad libitum food intake. Additionally, the changes in insulin concentrations during energy restriction were investigated in relation to compensatory energy and macronutrient intakes.


SUBJECTS AND METHODS  
Subjects
A total of 35 men [age: 18–50 y; body mass index (kg/m2): 20–30] were recruited among employees and students of Wageningen University, Wageningen, Netherlands. Only men were recruited to exclude an effect of menstrual cycles, which has been shown to affect both leptin concentrations (16) and appetite (17). Exclusion criteria for participation were as follows: a high level of dietary restraint (Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire restraint scale score > 2.38; 15); anemia (hemoglobin < 8.5 mmol/L, hematocrit < 0.41 L/L); diabetes or disturbed glucose metabolism (fasting plasma glucose concentration > 6.1 mmol/L or glucosuria; 18); use of medication that affects energy metabolism, weight, or food intake; blood donation during the intervention study; and stomach or bowel diseases (blood in stool, constipation, diarrhea). A total of 35 subjects participated in the study. One subject had a very low leptin concentration before the intervention (0.9 µg/mL) and, as a result, had a very high increase in leptin (65%) after energy restriction, rather than the expected decrease. Because of this change in leptin, we omitted the data for this outlying subject. The characteristics of the remaining 34 subjects are described in Table 1. Before screening, all participants gave their written informed consent. The Ethics Committee of Wageningen University approved the study protocol.


View this table:
TABLE 1. Subject characteristics of the men participating in the intervention study1

 
Energy-restriction period
Days 1 and 2 of the protocol were strictly controlled and consisted of an energy-restricted diet providing two-thirds of estimated energy needs. To calculate the individual energy needs of each subject, the basal metabolic rate was estimated by using the equation of Schofield (19):

RESULTS  
Energy restriction
At baseline, the serum leptin concentration was associated with body weight (r=0.39, P < 0.05) and body mass index (r=0.55, P < 0.001) but not with insulin concentration (r=0.18, P=0.30). The average change in body weight after energy restriction was –1.1 ± 0.7 kg (range: –2.6 to –0.3 kg), and serum leptin, serum insulin, and plasma glucose concentrations changed by –1.0 (95% CI: –1.4, –0.5) µg/mL, –2.4 (95% CI: –3.3, –1.5) µU/mL, and –0.3 (95% CI: –0.4, –0.2) mmol/L, respectively (Table 3). Decreases in fasting leptin concentrations were neither associated with changes in body weight (r=0.20, NS) nor with changes in insulin concentrations (r=0.21, NS) but were highly associated with fasting leptin concentrations at baseline (r=0.89, P < 0.0001), the proportion of energy restriction (r=0.48, P < 0.01), and baseline body mass index (r=0.44, P < 0.01). The decreases in fasting leptin concentrations were weakly associated with baseline body weight (r=0.33, P=0.05). During the energy-restriction period, the subjects consumed an average of 4.9 ± 0.5 MJ (range: 4.4–6.1 MJ), which reflected 38.0 ± 2.7% (range: 32.1–43.1%) of their estimated energy needs (Table 2).


View this table:
TABLE 3. Biochemical variables before intervention, after 2 d of 62% energy restriction, and after 2 d of ad libitum food intake1

 
Ad libitum food intake
During breakfast on day 3, 3.8 ± 1.2 MJ (range: 1.7–5.1 MJ) of the milk shake was consumed. In total, 18.3 ± 2.9 MJ (range: 12.6–26.2 MJ) was consumed on day 3, which corresponded with 143 ± 27% (range: 100–217%) of the estimated energy needs. On day 4, 16.0 ± 2.6 MJ (range: 11.0–20.7 MJ) was consumed, which was 124 ± 20% (range: 92–181%) of the estimated energy needs. The energy consumed on day 3 was positively associated with the degree of energy restriction (r=0.37, P < 0.05). During the ad libitum days, the subjects regained 1.2 ± 0.6 kg of body weight. Fasting glucose, insulin, and leptin concentrations increased significantly by 0.2 (95% CI: 0.1, 0.3) mmol/L, 2.5 (95% CI: 1.2, 3.7) µU/mL, and 0.7 (95% CI: 0.4, 0.9) µg/mL, respectively, relative to day 3 (Table 3). On average, fasting leptin concentrations increased by 37.6% (95% CI: 26.7, 48.5%), and insulin increased by 84.1% (95% CI: 55.9, 112.3%). These increases in fasting leptin and insulin concentrations were associated with their decreases during energy restriction: r=0.88 (P < 0.0001) and r=0.47 (P < 0.01), respectively.

Leptin and insulin decreases in relation to energy intake
Fasting serum leptin concentrations on day 3 were not associated with energy intake on the same day: energy intake in kJ (r=–0.11, P=0.55) and energy intake as a percentage of energy (r=–0.13, P=0.45). A small positive trend (r=0.31, P=0.08) was observed between the ratio of fasting leptin concentrations on day 3 and day 1 and energy intake (expressed as a percentage of estimated energy requirements) on the first ad libitum day (Table 4). In other words, a large decrease in leptin tended to be associated with a low energy intake directly after energy restriction (Figure 1). On the second ad libitum day, no such trend was observed (r=0.15, P=0.41). No statistically significant associations were observed between the ratio of fasting insulin concentrations on day 3 and day 1 and the ad libitum energy intake on day 3 (r=0.27, P=0.13) or day 4 (r=0.22, P=0.21).


View this table:
TABLE 4. Associations between changes in leptin and insulin in response to 2 d of 62% energy restriction and after 2 d of ad libitum food intake1

 

View larger version (15K):
FIGURE 1.. Scatter plot of the ratio of fasting leptin concentrations on day 3 to those on day 1 (before and after 62% energy restriction, respectively) and energy intake on the subsequent 2 d of an libitum diet (n=34).

 
Leptin and insulin decreases in relation to macronutrient composition
No statistically significant associations were observed between changes in leptin during energy restriction and macronutrient specific energy intake (Table 4). However, on the first ad libitum day, the ratio for fasting insulin concentrations was inversely associated with the percentage of energy derived from carbohydrates (r=–0.49, P=0.006). On day 4, this association became weaker (r=–0.30, P=0.08). In other words, a larger decrease in insulin was associated with a diet containing more energy from carbohydrate (Figure 2).


View larger version (15K):
FIGURE 2.. Scatter plot of the ratio of fasting insulin concentrations on day 3 to those on day 1 (before and after 62% energy restriction, respectively) and energy derived from carbohydrates on the subsequent 2 d of an ad libitum diet (n=34).

 

DISCUSSION  
Although it is generally accepted that acute decreases in leptin, induced by severe energy restriction, are signals to the brain to stimulate energy intake, the present study is the first to investigate this in 34 young adult men. In the present study, we observed that leptin concentrations decreased significantly and that subjects showed compensatory energy intake. However, we did not observe a statistically significant association between the magnitude of the decrease in leptin and energy intake compensation.

As expected, serum leptin concentrations decreased after energy restriction and increased after refeeding. Additionally, other variables—such as body weight, insulin, and glucose—also showed the expected pattern: a decrease during energy restriction and an increase during ad libitum intake. Moreover, subjects showed compensatory behavior during the ad libitum period, ie, they consumed considerably more energy than their estimated energy needs. Compensatory energy intake was especially apparent on day 3; the subjects consumed an average of 43% more than their estimated energy needs. Nevertheless, we did not observe that men with a large decrease in leptin concentration had a larger ad libitum energy intake than did men with a small decrease in leptin concentration.

It might be that we did not find an association between the magnitude of the decrease in fasting leptin concentration and energy intake compensation because our group of subjects was too homogeneous. As stated in the introduction, restraint eating, ie, a high cognitive awareness of food intake (15), and being overweight may suppress voluntary energy intake after weight loss and therefore affect energy intake compensation. For this reason, we selected a group of lean unrestrained-eating men for our intervention. However, by selecting this homogenous group, we presumably narrowed the variation in the leptin response. This may have resulted in a small contrast in energy intake compensation between the so-called hyporesponders and hyperresponders, thereby underestimating the existing associations.

During the ad libitum period, we used 2 methods to make the subjects aware of their internal cues for appetite and to prevent them from consuming their habitual meals. First, we provided the foods in unusual portion sizes, ie, greater or smaller portions (appendix A). Second, the breakfast on day 3 consisted of a milk shake, for which the energy content was not known to the subjects. It is possible that these 2 methods might have introduced extra variation in energy intake. However, we believe that these methods were necessary to make the subjects alert to their internal cues for appetite.

In our study, the energy intake compensation was defined as the proportional energy intake compared with the estimated energy needs. It might have been that the estimation of the energy needs affected this outcome measure. We used the equation of Schofield in combination with a physical activity questionnaire to estimate individual energy needs. The Schofield equation is a simple equation based on the sex, age, and weight of the subject (19), and it has been shown that over- or underestimation with these type of equations is more likely to occur in obese subjects than in lean subjects, as in our study (23). The physical activity questionnaire that we used included questions on 6 daily activities (20), of which the number of hours of sleep and hours of working or studying were the most important questions. These 2 activities are rather constant within a subject. Moreover, the physical activity levels we observed were similar to those found in other studies (: 1.8; range: 1.5–2.2) ( It has been shown that leptin has a diurnal pattern, which peaks at night (25–27). Several studies have shown that, during energy restriction, not only fasting leptin concentrations, but also the diurnal variation of these concentrations, is decreased (26, 27). Additionally, several studies have shown that carbohydrate composition affects the diurnal pattern of leptin. Weigle et al (28) have observed that a high-carbohydrate, low-fat diet increased the amplitude of leptin. Moreover, they observed that this increase in amplitude was predictive for the degree of body weight and fat lost during this study. This is in contrast with the finding of Teff et al (29), who observed that fructose-containing beverages decreased the amplitude of leptin compared with glucose-containing drinks, resulting in an increase in energy derived from an ad libitum test meal in restrained-eating women. In our study design, the measurement of spontaneous energy intake was critical. Continuous or multiple blood sampling would require a medical setting, which may have affected spontaneous energy intake. We therefore chose to measure ad libitum energy intake as accurately as possible in a free-living situation, but had to make the concession to only measure fasting leptin concentrations. However, further studies should also consider the change in diurnal variation as an important starvation signal.

We observed that large decreases in insulin lead to a high carbohydrate intake, ie, energy derived from carbohydrates and the amount of mono- and disaccharides that was consumed. This is in contrast with a classic study by Rodin et al (30), which observed that not low, but high, insulin concentrations increase hunger, palatability of sucrose, sweetness, and food intake. However, this study investigated hyperinsulemic clamps under short-term (150 min) experimental conditions, which are not comparable with our study setting.

There is evidence that the decrease in insulin during energy restriction might induce the decrease in circulating leptin (5, 7). During energy restriction, glycogen stores are being depleted, insulin concentrations decrease, and the body switches over to fatty acid oxidation. Human and in vitro studies have shown that a decreased glucose metabolism results in a reduction in leptin production (31, 32). It has therefore been suggested that foods with a high glycemic index during energy restriction may blunt the decreases in leptin (33, 34) and thus lower the starvation signal to the brain, which eventually prevents weight regain after weight loss. However, others speculate that carbohydrates with a low glycemic index, ie, fructose, have the opposite effect; they do not stimulate the diurnal variation in leptin and basal leptin concentrations, which results in an increase in energy intake (29).

In our study, few data on the specific mono- and disaccharide contents of the carbohydrates were available; only 3–16% of the data were available in the Dutch nutrient database (22, 35). Because no reliable and accurate data are available, we can only speculate that the carbohydrate composition of the energy-restricted diet might have affected the leptin response and, therefore, the intake of carbohydrates during the ad libitum period.

Several human studies have observed that a greater decrease in leptin is associated with a greater appetite (10–13). However, no data are yet available on actual energy intake. Although it has been shown that the level of appetite is a good predictor of energy intake (36–38), it is possible that the relation between the motivation to eat and actual food intake is disturbed by a cognitive awareness in a human situation. This may affect food intake even after the exclusion of restrained-eating subjects from participation. The awareness of the subjects being on an energy-restricted diet might have disturbed the association between internal cues, ie, the decrease in fasting leptin, and subsequent energy intake compensation.

Animal models still provide most of the direct evidence for the role of the acute leptin response in energy intake compensation after energy restriction (6). It might be that the role of leptin in starvation and energy intake regulation is different in rodents and cannot be extrapolated to humans. First, leptin-deficient rodents and starving rodents show several neuroendocrine changes (1), which are not observed in leptin-deficient humans (39–41) or fasting humans (42). These observations stress that there might be important differences in the physiology of starvation between species. Second, the cognitive awareness of humans during energy restriction, which is always present, also makes it very difficult to extrapolate findings from rodents to humans.

Overall, we conclude that the decreases in fasting leptin concentrations induced by a 62% energy-restricted diet of 2 d was not associated with the energy intake compensation during the following 2 d. Although it is generally accepted that the acute response in fasting leptin after energy restriction is a starvation response, more studies have to be conducted to confirm this assumption. Controlled interventions in obese and nonobese subjects are especially needed to unravel the function of this starvation response in humans.


ACKNOWLEDGMENTS  
We thank the volunteers who participated in this study; Els Siebelink, Karin Borgonjen, and Lieneke Kolmus for their help during the preparation and implementation of the intervention study; and Lucy Ockma and coworkers for their assistance in the sample collection.

MM helped design the study, collected data, analyzed the statistical data, and wrote the manuscript. CdG, LCPMdG, and FJK helped design the study and provided significant advice and consultation. No conflicts of interest were declared by any of the authors.


REFERENCES  

  1. Zhang Y, Proenca R, Maffei M, Barone M, Leopold L, Friedman JM. Positional cloning of the mouse obese gene and its human homologue. Nature 1994;372:425–32.
  2. Considine RV, Sinha MK, Heiman ML, et al. Serum immunoreactive-leptin concentrations in normal-weight and obese humans. N Engl J Med 1996;334:292–5.
  3. Kolaczynski JW, Considine RV, Ohannesian J, et al. Responses of leptin to short-term fasting and refeeding in humans: a link with ketogenesis but not ketones themselves. Diabetes 1996;45:1511–5.
  4. Haluzik M, Matoulek M, Svacina S, Hilgertova J, Haas T. The influence of short-term fasting on serum leptin levels, and selected hormonal and metabolic parameters in morbidly obese and lean females. Endocr Res 2001;27:251–60.
  5. Dubuc GR, Phinney SD, Stern JS, Havel PJ. Changes of serum leptin and endocrine and metabolic parameters after 7 days of energy restriction in men and women. Metabolism 1998;47:429–34.
  6. Flier JS. Obesity wars: molecular progress confronts an expanding epidemic. Cell 2004;116:337–50.
  7. Boden G, Chen X, Mozzoli M, Ryan I. Effect of fasting on serum leptin in normal human subjects. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1996;81:3419–23.
  8. Baskin DG, Figlewicz Lattemann D, Seeley RJ, Woods SC, Porte D Jr, Schwartz MW. Insulin and leptin: dual adiposity signals to the brain for the regulation of food intake and body weight. Brain Res 1999;848:114–23.
  9. Niswender KD, Schwartz MW. Insulin and leptin revisited: adiposity signals with overlapping physiological and intracellular signaling capabilities. Front Neuroendocrinol 2003;24:1–10.
  10. Heini AF, Lara-Castro C, Kirk KA, Considine RV, Caro JF, Weinsier RL. Association of leptin and hunger-satiety ratings in obese women. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 1998;22:1084–7.
  11. Keim NL, Stern JS, Havel PJ. Relation between circulating leptin concentrations and appetite during a prolonged, moderate energy deficit in women. Am J Clin Nutr 1998;68:794–801.
  12. Westerterp-Plantenga MS, Saris WH, Hukshorn CJ, Campfield LA. Effects of weekly administration of pegylated recombinant human OB protein on appetite profile and energy metabolism in obese men. Am J Clin Nutr 2001;74:426–34.
  13. Mars M, de Graaf C, Van-Rossum C, De-Groot C, Seidell J, Kok F. Leptin and appetite responses induced by a four-day energy restriction: preliminary results. Appetite 2002;39:247(abstr).
  14. Chin-Chance C, Polonsky KS, Schoeller DA. Twenty-four-hour leptin levels respond to cumulative short-term energy imbalance and predict subsequent intake. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2000;85:2685–91.
  15. Van-Strien T. Eating behaviour personality traits and body mass. Wageningen, Netherlands: Landbouw Hogeschool, 1986.
  16. Buffenstein R, Poppitt SD, McDevitt RM, Prentice AM. Food intake and the menstrual cycle: a retrospective analysis, with implications for appetite research. Physiol Behav 1995;58:1067–77.
  17. Riad-Gabriel MG, Jinagouda SD, Sharma A, Boyadjian R, Saad MF. Changes in plasma leptin during the menstrual cycle. Eur J Endocrinol 1998;139:528–31.
  18. WHO. Prevention of diabetes mellitus. Geneva: World Health Organization, 1994.
  19. James W, Schofield E. Different levels of analysis in estimating requirements. In: James WPT, Schofield E, eds. Human energy requirements. 1st ed. New York, NY: Oxford Medical Publications, 1990:42–57.
  20. WHO. Principles for the estimation of energy requirements. Energy and protein requirements. Report of a joint FAO/WHO/UNU expert consultation. 2nd ed. Geneva: World Health Organization, 1985:34–52.
  21. Voorlichtingsbureau-voor-de-voeding. Zo eet Nederland 1998, resultaten van de voedselconsumptiepeiling 1998. (National food consumption data 1998.) Den Haag, Netherlands: Voorlichtingsbureau voor de voeding, 1998 (in Dutch.)
  22. Stichting-Nederlands-Voedingsstoffenbestand. Nederlands voedingsstoffen bestand NEVO tabel 1996. (Dutch nutrient data base 1996.) Den Haag, Netherlands: Voorlichtingsbureau voor de voeding, 1998 (in Dutch).
  23. Frankenfield DC, Rowe WA, Smith JS, Cooney RN. Validation of several established equations for resting metabolic rate in obese and nonobese people. J Am Diet Assoc 2003;103:1152–9.
  24. Black AE. Physical activity levels from a meta-analysis of doubly labeled water studies for validating energy intake as measured by dietary assessment. Nutr Rev 1996;54:170–4.
  25. Sinha MK, Ohannesian JP, Heiman ML, et al. Nocturnal rise of leptin in lean, obese, and non-insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus subjects. J Clin Invest 1996;97:1344–7.
  26. Hilton LK, Loucks AB. Low energy availability, not exercise stress, suppresses the diurnal rhythm of leptin in healthy young women. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 2000;278:E43–9.
  27. Elimam A, Marcus C. Meal timing, fasting and glucocorticoids interplay in serum leptin concentrations and diurnal profile. Eur J Endocrinol 2002;147:181–8.
  28. Weigle DS, Cummings DE, Newby PD, et al. Roles of leptin and ghrelin in the loss of body weight caused by a low fat, high carbohydrate diet. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2003;88:1577–86.
  29. Teff KL, Elliott SS, Tschop M, et al. Dietary fructose reduces circulating insulin and leptin, attenuates postprandial suppression of ghrelin, and increases triglycerides in women. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2004;89:2963–72.
  30. Rodin J, Wack J, Ferrannini E, DeFronzo RA. Effect of insulin and glucose on feeding behavior. Metabolism 1985;34:826–31.
  31. Mueller WM, Gregoire FM, Stanhope KL, et al. Evidence that glucose metabolism regulates leptin secretion from cultured rat adipocytes. Endocrinology 1998;139:551–8.
  32. Wellhoener P, Fruehwald-Schultes B, Kern W, et al. Glucose metabolism rather than insulin is a main determinant of leptin secretion in humans. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2000;85:1267–71.
  33. Jenkins AB, Markovic TP, Fleury A, Campbell LV. Carbohydrate intake and short-term regulation of leptin in humans. Diabetologia 1997;40:348–51.
  34. Agus MS, Swain JF, Larson CL, Eckert EA, Ludwig DS. Dietary composition and physiologic adaptations to energy restriction. Am J Clin Nutr 2000;71:901–7.
  35. Stichting-Nederlands-Voedingsstoffenbestand. Nederlands voedingsstoffen bestand NEVO tabel 2001. (Dutch nutrient data base 2001.) The Hague: Voorlichtingsbureau voor de voeding, 2001(in Dutch).
  36. Flint A, Raben A, Blundell JE, Astrup A. Reproducibility, power and validity of visual analogue scales in assessment of appetite sensations in single test meal studies. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 2000;24:38–48.
  37. De Graaf C. The validity of appetite ratings. Appetite 1993;21:156–60.
  38. Stubbs RJ, Hughes DA, Johnstone AM, et al. The use of visual analogue scales to assess motivation to eat in human subjects: a review of their reliability and validity with an evaluation of new hand-held computerized systems for temporal tracking of appetite ratings. Br J Nutr 2000;84:405–15.
  39. O’Rahilly S. Leptin: defining its role in humans by the clinical study of genetic disorders. Nutr Rev 2002;60:S30–4; discussion S68–84, 85–7.
  40. Montague CT, Farooqi IS, Whitehead JP, et al. Congenital leptin deficiency is associated with severe early-onset obesity in humans. Nature 1997;387:903–8.
  41. Ahima RS, Prabakaran D, Mantzoros C, et al. Role of leptin in the neuroendocrine response to fasting. Nature 1996;382:250–2.
  42. Chan JL, Heist K, DePaoli AM, Veldhuis JD, Mantzoros CS. The role of falling leptin levels in the neuroendocrine and metabolic adaptation to short-term starvation in healthy men. J Clin Invest 2003;111:1409–21.
Received for publication March 2, 2004. Accepted for publication November 10, 2004.


作者: Monica Mars
医学百科App—中西医基础知识学习工具
  • 相关内容
  • 近期更新
  • 热文榜
  • 医学百科App—健康测试工具