Literature
首页医源资料库在线期刊美国临床营养学杂志2005年82卷第5期

Functional magnetic resonance imaging of human hypothalamic responses to sweet taste and calories

来源:《美国临床营养学杂志》
摘要:Previously,wefoundaprolongeddose-dependentdecreaseinthehypothalamicfunctionalmagneticresonanceimagingsignalafteringestionofaglucosesolution。Objectives:Theaimsofthepresentstudyweretomeasuretheeffectsofsweettasteandenergycontentonthehypothalamicresponseto......

点击显示 收起

Paul AM Smeets, Cees de Graaf, Annette Stafleu, Matthias JP van Osch and Jeroen van der Grond

1 From the Image Sciences Institute, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, Netherlands (PAMS, MJPvO, and JvdG) and the Department of Food and Chemical Risk Analysis, TNO Quality of Life, Zeist, Netherlands (PAMS, CdG, and AS)

2 Supported by the Dutch Ministry of Education, Culture, and Science and the Dutch Ministry of Health, Welfare, and Sport.

3 Reprints not available. Address correspondence to PAM Smeets, University Medical Center Utrecht, the Image Sciences Institute, Heidelberglaan 100, room QS 459, 3584 CX Utrecht, Netherlands. E-mail: paul{at}isi.uu.nl


ABSTRACT  
Background: Evidence exists that beverages do not trigger appropriate anticipatory physiologic responses, such as cephalic phase insulin release. Therefore, it is of interest to elucidate the food properties necessary for triggering adaptive responses. Previously, we found a prolonged dose-dependent decrease in the hypothalamic functional magnetic resonance imaging signal after ingestion of a glucose solution.

Objectives: The aims of the present study were to measure the effects of sweet taste and energy content on the hypothalamic response to glucose ingestion and to measure the concomitant changes in blood glucose and insulin concentrations.

Design: Five healthy, normal-weight men participated in a randomized crossover design trial. The subjects were scanned 4 times for 37 min on separate days with functional magnetic resonance imaging. After 7 min, they ingested 1 of the following 4 stimuli (300 mL of each): water (control), a glucose solution, an aspartame (sweet taste) solution, or a maltodextrin (nonsweet carbohydrate) solution.

Results: Glucose ingestion resulted in a prolonged and significant signal decrease in the upper hypothalamus (P < 0.05). Water, aspartame, and maltodextrin had no such effect. Glucose and maltodextrin ingestions resulted in similar increases in blood glucose and insulin concentrations. However, only glucose triggered an early rise in insulin concentrations. Aspartame did not trigger any insulin response.

Conclusions: Our findings suggest that both sweet taste and energy content are required for a hypothalamic response. The combination of sweet taste and energy content could be crucial in triggering adaptive responses to sweetened beverages.

Key Words: Functional magnetic resonance imaging • hypothalamus • glucose • insulin • aspartame • maltodextrin • taste • carbohydrates • men


INTRODUCTION  
Beverages are an increasingly important source of carbohydrate intake (1, 2). Unfortunately, consumption of caloric beverages does not elicit appropriate dietary compensation and leads to excessive energy intake (1, 3, 4), which promotes obesity. A likely cause for this excessive energy intake is that beverages cause less sensory stimulation because they are swallowed and not chewed and therefore lack the olfactory and visual stimulation of normal foods. Sensory stimulation, such as the sight, smell, and taste of food, can trigger anticipatory physiologic responses, also called cephalic phase responses, which improve the digestion and absorption of nutrients (5-7). In view of the prevalent consumption of beverages, it is of interest to elucidate the properties of foods that are necessary for triggering these adaptive responses. Some evidence exists that liquids do not induce cephalic phase responses; for example, tasting sweet liquids does not elicit cephalic phase insulin release (CPIR) (8, 9), which is important for the prevention of peaks in blood glucose (10-12). In contrast, tasting food can trigger CPIR (5, 13) and so can the combination of sweet taste and the presentation of a meal (8). Recently, we reported a prolonged dose-dependent decrease in the blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) signal in the hypothalamus a few minutes after the ingestion of a glucose solution (14). BOLD functional MRI (fMRI) measures changes in neuronal activity levels based on the associated changes in the local concentrations of oxygenated and deoxygenated hemoglobin (15, 16). We suggested that the initial part of this response could be triggered by the sweet taste of the glucose solution and could be associated with changes in the blood insulin concentration during the cephalic phase (ie, before glucose has entered the blood stream). The subsequent rise in blood glucose and insulin concentrations could relate to the additional hypothalamic response (14). The aim of the present study was to investigate the separate effects of sweet taste and energy content on this hypothalamic response as well as on blood glucose and insulin concentrations.


SUBJECTS AND METHODS  
Subjects
Five healthy, normal-weight men participated in the present study [ Experimental procedures
We used a randomized crossover design with 4 stimuli: water, water + glucose, water + aspartame, and water + maltodextrin (a nonsweet glucose dimer). Conditions were randomly assigned to the subjects by drawing lots the day before every visit. The order of treatment is shown in Table 1. The sweetness of the aspartame solution and energy-content of the maltodextrin solution were matched to those of the glucose solution. The details and preparation of the stimuli were as follows: 300-mL tap water was used for water, the 75-g glucose solution was obtained by dissolving 82.5 g glucose monohydrate (D-dextrose; Avebe Corporate, Veendam, Netherlands) in water to a volume of 300 mL, the equally sweet aspartame solution was obtained by dissolving 880 mg pure aspartame (Aspartamum; BUFA Corporate, Uitgeest, Netherlands) in 300 mL water, and the equicaloric maltodextrin solution was obtained by dissolving 78.9 g Fantomalt (Nutricia, Netherlands) in water to a volume of 300 mL (300 kcal). The amount of aspartame that was needed to match the sweetness of the 75-g glucose solution was estimated from the data of the PhD dissertations of de Graaf (17) and Schifferstein (18). A pilot test, in which 5 volunteers were asked to assess the sweetness of the 2 solutions in a blind fashion, confirmed that this determination was correct. The subjects were blinded as much as possible; all stimuli looked identical and the subjects did not know in advance the stimulus they would receive. When asked afterward, the subjects were unable to discern between the glucose and aspartame solutions. The maltodextrin solution was unknown to the subjects, and most of the subjects reported a slight starch taste, which made them guess it was maltodextrin. Water was recognized by the subjects as such.


View this table:
TABLE 1. . Order of treatment for every subject 1

 
The subjects fasted (no food or beverages, except water) overnight from 2200 and were scanned the next morning, starting between 0930 and 1000. The scans were performed on 4 separate days 1 wk apart with a 1.5-T Philips Gyroscan ACS-NT system (Philips Medical Systems, Best, Netherlands). The subjects were placed in a supine position and their heads were immobilized with a vacuum cushion that was designed for use in a MRI head coil (Medical Intelligence, Schwabmünchen, Germany). The functional scan consisted of a T2*-weighted gradient-echo, segmented echo-planar imaging sequence [repetition time = 120 ms, echo time = 40 ms, flip angle = 30°, image matrix = 198 x 256, field of view = 208 x 208 mm, 12 signal averages/scan, 33 k-lines acquired/excitation pulse (14)] with which a 10-mm thick midsagittal slice was scanned. The images were reconstructed to 256 x 256 pixels. The subjects were scanned for 37 min (256 scans). After a reference period (baseline) of 7.2 min (50 scans), the subjects ingested one of the test solutions through a peroral tube. After the functional scan, a T1-weighted anatomical scan was made of the same slice (repetition time= 600 ms, echo time = 18 ms, field of view = 230 x 230 mm).

Blood sampling and analysis
On every test day, 10 blood samples were drawn with 5-mL syringes in combination with a canula that was placed in an antecubital vein; one sample was taken before scanning and 9 samples were taken during scanning at –5 and –3 min (before stimulus ingestion) and at 1, 3, 5, 7, 10, 20, and 29 min after the onset of drinking. The samples were immediately injected into 2-mL K-Oxalate/Na-Fl tubes. The tubes were kept on ice until the experiment ended and were then centrifuged at 1730 x g for 10 min at 4°C. Blood plasma was stored in aliquots at –80°C until analyzed. Sample handling and analysis were performed by U-diagnostics Corporate (Utrecht, Netherlands; an ISO 9002-certified laboratory). Glucose concentrations were measured with the glucose-oxidase method on a VITROS 250 Chemistry System (Johnson & Johnson Clinical Diagnostics, Rochester, NY). Insulin concentrations were measured with an immunoassay (AxSYM insulin assay; Abbott, Abbott Park, IL).

Data preprocessing
fMRI data of the hypothalamus were preprocessed and analyzed as described previously (14). All 256 functional images of each time series were motion-corrected with the Multimodality Image Registration using Information Theory software for image registration by maximization of mutual information (19). The images were aligned to the middle image and the anatomical T1-weighted image was also coregistered with this image.

Data analysis
For every subject, the hypothalamus was manually segmented with the use of the anatomical image and divided into 4 regions by 2 orthogonal axes according to predefined criteria (20): the upper anterior hypothalamus (UAH), lower anterior hypothalamus, upper posterior hypothalamus (UPH), and lower posterior hypothalamus. The anterior–posterior axis was defined by the line passing through the centers of the anterior commissure and the mammillary body. The upper–lower axis was defined by the line passing through the optic chiasm, which is perpendicular to the anterior–posterior axis (Figure 1). The UAH and UPH were the regions of interest (ROIs) because these regions respond to glucose (14). In addition, a square reference area (10 x 10 pixels) of about the same size as the hypothalamus was delineated in the frontal cortex, anterior of the genu of the corpus callosum. At every time point, the mean gray value in the hypothalamus as a whole and in each ROI was calculated. Next, these mean gray values were normalized to the mean of their 7-min baseline value, which yielded the percentage signal change from mean baseline. The global signal drift (scanner drift) was corrected by subtracting the mean signal in the reference area from those in the ROIs. We tested for an effect of every stimulus using differential regression analysis (21), in which the mean signal changes per minute were compared with the mean signal change during the 7-min reference period using Student's t tests. Because drinking causes artifacts (14), data from the first 2 min after the onset of drinking were excluded from the analysis. A Bonferroni-corrected threshold of P = 0.0018 was employed because we performed 28 t tests per condition.


View larger version (108K):
FIGURE 1.. Segmentation and subdivision of the hypothalamus into 2 regions of interest (20). Ac, anterior commissure; mm, mammillary body; oc, optic chiasm; UA, upper anterior hypothalamus; UP, upper posterior hypothalamus.

 
To compare treatment effects, the area under the curves (AUCs) were calculated for every subject from 2 to 30 min after stimulus ingestion in the UAH and the UPH (193 time points). For the blood data, the AUCs above baseline (ie, the first measurement) were calculated for every subject. The AUCs were calculated with a trapezoidal integration. With the use of randomized block designs (analysis of variance), the AUCs of the fMRI data and the blood glucose and insulin data were tested for an overall effect of treatment. The AUCs of the fMRI data were also tested for an effect of ROI. If a treatment effect was observed, post hoc tests were performed to compare treatments. For the fMRI data, we used Dunnett's t test to compare all other treatments with water (control condition) and Tukey's honestly significant difference test to compare the treatments pairwise. For the glucose and insulin data, we compared all treatments pairwise using Tukey's honestly significant difference test. Statistical analyses were done with SPSS statistical software (version 12.0.1; SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL). A P value < 0.05 (two-sided and corrected for multiple comparisons when appropriate) was considered significant.


RESULTS  
Hypothalamic fMRI signal
The fMRI signal changes in the UAH and the UPH are shown in Figure 2. The P values from the Student's t tests that compared every time block with the reference period are shown below that. Glucose ingestion resulted in a prolonged significant decrease in signal compared with baseline in both ROIs; the decrease started 2–5 min after the onset of drinking and lasted for the remainder of the scan (30 min). Water, aspartame, and maltodextrin had no such effect on the hypothalamic signal. Maltodextrin showed some significant signal changes in the UPH, but these were not consistent. An analysis of variance of the AUCs uncovered a significant effect of treatment but no significant effect of ROIs. The AUCs for all treatments in the upper hypothalamus are shown in Figure 3. The AUC of glucose was significantly different from that of water. Otherwise, the AUCs did not significantly differ from each other. Thus, only glucose affected the hypothalamic fMRI response.


View larger version (34K):
FIGURE 2.. Mean (±SEM) functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) responses in 5 men in the fasting state and after ingestion of four 300-mL test substances. , water; , water + 75 g glucose (G); •, water + aspartame (A); , water + maltodextrin (M). Top: signal changes from baseline in the upper anterior hypothalamus (left panel) and the upper posterior hypothalamus (right panel) during the 7-min reference period and in the 28 min after stimulus ingestion. The vertical bars labeled "drinking" indicate the approximate period of stimulus ingestion (2 min). Bottom: P values of the Student's t tests that compared the mean signal per minute with the mean signal during the reference period. Signal changes during the reference period are near zero. Mean (±SEM) reference in the upper anterior hypothalamus: –0.032 ± 0.050 (water), –0.003 ± 0.045 (G), –0.011 ± 0.065 (A), –0.033 ± 0.048 (M). Mean (±SEM) reference in the upper posterior hypothalamus: 0.025 ± 0.055 (water), –0.003 ± 0.046 (G), –0.009 ± 0.066 (A), –0.020 ± 0.069 (M). The dashed lines indicate the Bonferroni-corrected threshold of P = 0.0018. After glucose ingestion, significant decreases in fMRI signal in the upper anterior hypothalamus and upper posterior hypothalamus were observed.

 

View larger version (9K):
FIGURE 3.. Mean (±SEM) area under the curve (AUC) of the fMRI response in the upper hypothalamus (UH) of 5 men. The AUCs of the 2 hypothalamic regions of interest were combined because an analysis of variance showed no significant effect of the regions of interest on AUC. G, glucose; A, aspartame; M, maltodextrin. A significant effect of treatment was observed on the AUC, P = 0.005. Bars with different letters are significantly different, P < 0.05 (Dunnett's t test in a comparison with water and Tukey's honestly significant difference in pairwise comparisons of treatments).

 
Blood measurements
Plasma concentrations and AUCs of glucose and insulin are shown in Figure 4. Energy intake (glucose and maltodextrin) resulted in increased glucose and insulin concentrations starting 5–10 min after the onset of ingestion. Similar to water, aspartame (ie, sweet taste) did not affect glucose and insulin concentrations. An early rise in insulin concentration was seen only after glucose ingestion, 5 min after the onset of drinking. At this time, no rise in plasma glucose is yet observed. Analyses of variance showed that significant effects of treatment on glucose and insulin concentrations existed. The AUCs of glucose and maltodextrin were not significantly different, but were significantly greater than those of water and aspartame.


View larger version (17K):
FIGURE 4.. Mean (±SEM) responses of plasma glucose and insulin in 5 men in the fasting state (3 time points) and after ingestion of four 300-mL test substances (7 time points). AUC, area under the curve above baseline. Left: , water; , water + 75 g glucose (G); •, water + aspartame (A); , water + maltodextrin (M). The time point t = 0 corresponds to the onset of ingestion. Right: Significant effect of treatment on the AUCs of glucose and insulin, P < 0.05 (ANOVA). Bars in the same panel with different letters are significantly different, P < 0.05 (Tukey's honestly significant difference).

 

DISCUSSION  
We investigated the effects of sweet taste and energy content on the hypothalamic response to glucose ingestion and on the concomitant changes in blood glucose and insulin concentrations. Glucose ingestion resulted in a prolonged signal decrease in the upper hypothalamus. Water, aspartame, and maltodextrin had no such effect. Ingestion of either glucose or maltodextrin resulted in similar increases in blood glucose and insulin concentrations. However, only glucose triggered an early rise in plasma insulin. The sweet taste of aspartame did not trigger any insulin response.

The prolonged signal decrease we observed in the UAH and UPH after the ingestion of a 75-g glucose solution was similar to the response we had previously reported but with a smaller amplitude (14). Both the aspartame and maltodextrin ingestions were not associated with significant changes in the hypothalamic signal. Apparently, neither sweet taste nor energy content was sufficient to trigger a response. This suggests that a hypothalamic response requires both sweet taste and energy content.

The aspartame and maltodextrin ingestions did not result in early changes in the insulin concentration. Our finding that swallowing an aspartame-sweetened liquid does not elicit CPIR adds to reports of the absence of CPIR in response to tasting (not swallowing) aspartame-sweetened liquids (8, 9). In the study by Teff et al (9), tasting saccharine as well as sucrose solutions also did not induce CPIR. Moreover, Bruce et al (8) showed that only the combination of sweet taste with the presentation of a meal induced CPIR. Thus, although one would expect the existence of conditioned adaptive responses after the ingestion of sweet stimuli, these studies suggest that, in general, sweet liquids do not induce CPIR, presumably because they do not cause sufficient sensory stimulation. CPIR was not observed in our study, and although this agrees with the findings of other studies, it could have been due to our small sample size (n = 5) and to the high variability in glucose and insulin concentrations, especially after the ingestion of carbohydrates. Interestingly, Grill et al (22) found a preeminent role of glucose in triggering CPIR in rats; other sugars (ie, sucrose and fructose) and a nonnutritive sweetener (sodium saccharine) did not elicit CPIR, whereas glucose did. They note that this is particularly striking, because glucose is neither the most intense nor the most palatable substance that was studied.

The maltodextrin solution, a nonsweet energy-rich liquid, did not elicit CPIR or a hypothalamic response. The absence of CPIR is not surprising given the sort of sensory stimulation it caused (a nonsweet watery taste). Also, the subjects were unfamiliar with this particular substance and thus were not conditioned to this combination of taste and energy content.

We had previously hypothesized that the early decrease in the hypothalamic signal, which was observed <5 min after glucose ingestion, was associated with CPIR (14). Our insulin data showed that only glucose was associated with an early rise in insulin concentration, and only glucose triggered a decrease in fMRI signal in the upper hypothalamus. This supports our hypothesis. In addition, we hypothesized that the additional decrease in fMRI signal was associated with a rise in insulin concentration. However, because maltodextrin causes an insulin response similar to that of glucose but no significant change in fMRI signal, this hypothesis is not supported. Taken together, our data show no direct relation between changes in the hypothalamic fMRI signal and the plasma insulin concentration. It is striking, however, that only glucose ingestion was associated with a hypothalamic response as well as an early insulin response. This raises the question whether this hypothalamic response is specifically triggered by glucose. Our present findings in humans and the findings of Grill et al (22) in rats suggest a particular sensitivity of the digestive system to glucose. It remains to be investigated whether other sweet carbohydrates, such as sucrose, induce a hypothalamic response.

We found a similar but smaller hypothalamic response after glucose ingestion than was previously reported (14). A possible reason for this is the stress caused by the blood sampling; otherwise, the experimental setup of both studies was the same. Stress could influence BOLD measurements of the hypothalamus in 2 ways. First, stress can affect responses of the hypothalamus because its paraventricular nucleus plays a major role in generating adaptive autonomic, behavioral, and hormonal responses to stress (23, 24). Second, stress could affect the BOLD signal. Acute stress has been shown to cause changes in hematocrit (25), which affects the source of the BOLD signal. It has been suggested that the stress caused by MRIs is a source of variation in hematocrit and therefore of variation in the BOLD signal (26); in our case, however, both experiments involved the same MRI protocol. Still, the added stress of the blood sampling procedures could have affected hematocrit. In addition, the effect of venipuncture on cortisol concentrations, a measure of stress, has been shown to be highly variable (27, 28), which suggests that the effects of stress on the fMRI response will vary between subjects, too. Therefore, the assessment of stress in future MRI experiments is warranted, especially when the experiments involve blood sampling; stress should be measured with salivary cortisol measurements, which allow for an unstressed reference measurement, and with a stress questionnaire to assess the subjective stress of each person. This also suggests that future studies could benefit from a larger sample size, given the small effect size seen in the present study.

In conclusion, we found that sweet taste and energy content on their own did not trigger a prolonged decrease in hypothalamic signal similar to that observed after glucose ingestion. Also, the sweet taste of aspartame was not sufficient to trigger an early insulin response. An early rise in plasma insulin concentration was observed after glucose ingestion, but not after maltodextrin ingestion. It is of interest whether the fMRI signal decrease we observed in the hypothalamus is specifically triggered by glucose. Our findings suggest that the combination of taste (sweetness) and energy content might be important in triggering adaptive physiologic responses to liquid stimuli, such as soft drinks. Moreover, a matching combination of sweet taste and energy content could be necessary for an adequate regulation of energy intake.


ACKNOWLEDGMENTS  
PAMS contributed to the experimental design, conducting the experiment, data analysis and interpretation, and writing of the manuscript. CdG contributed to the experimental design, interpretation of the data, and reviewing of the manuscript. AS contributed to the experimental design and reviewing of the manuscript. MJPvO contributed to the experimental design, gave advice with regard to the practical experimental setup, and reviewed the manuscript. JvdG contributed to the experimental design, conducting the experiment, and reviewing of the manuscript. None of the authors has any conflicts of interest.


REFERENCES  

  1. DiMeglio DP, Mattes RD. Liquid versus solid carbohydrate: effects on food intake and body weight. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 2000;24:794–800.
  2. Bray GA. The epidemic of obesity and changes in food intake: the Fluoride Hypothesis. Physiol Behav 2004;82:115–21.
  3. Mattes RD. Dietary compensation by humans for supplemental energy provided as ethanol or carbohydrate in fluids. Physiol Behav 1996;59:179–87.
  4. de Castro JM. The effects of the spontaneous ingestion of particular foods or beverages on the meal pattern and overall nutrient intake of humans. Physiol Behav 1993;53:1133–44.
  5. Rodin J. Insulin levels, hunger, and food intake: an example of feedback loops in body weight regulation. Health Psychol 1985;4:1–24.
  6. Mattes RD. Physiologic responses to sensory stimulation by food: nutritional implications. J Am Diet Assoc 1997;97:406–13.
  7. Teff K. Nutritional implications of the cephalic-phase reflexes: endocrine responses. Appetite 2000;34:206–13.
  8. Bruce DG, Storlien LH, Furler SM, Chisholm DJ. Cephalic phase metabolic responses in normal weight adults. Metabolism 1987;36:721–5.
  9. Teff KL, Devine J, Engelman K. Sweet taste: effect on cephalic phase insulin release in men. Physiol Behav 1995;57:1089–95.
  10. Ahren B, Holst JJ. The cephalic insulin response to meal ingestion in humans is dependent on both cholinergic and noncholinergic mechanisms and is important for postprandial glycemia. Diabetes 2001;50:1030–8.
  11. Harju E, Nordback I. Effect of atropine on insulin secretion in healthy subjects. J Int Med Res 1987;15:167–9.
  12. Kraegen EW, Chisholm DJ, McNamara ME. Timing of insulin delivery with meals. Horm Metab Res 1981;13:365–7.
  13. Teff KL, Mattes RD, Engelman K. Cephalic phase insulin release in normal weight males: verification and reliability. Am J Physiol 1991;261:E430–6.
  14. Smeets PAM, van Osch MJP, de Graaf C, Stafleu A, van der Grond J. Functional MRI of human hypothalamic responses following glucose ingestion. Neuroimage 2005;24:363–8.
  15. Kwong KK, Belliveau JW, Chesler DA, et al. Dynamic magnetic resonance imaging of human brain activity during primary sensory stimulation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1992;89:5675–9.
  16. Ogawa S, Tank DW, Menon R, et al. Intrinsic signal changes accompanying sensory stimulation: functional brain mapping with magnetic resonance imaging. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1992;89:5951–5.
  17. de Graaf C. Psychophysical studies of mixtures of tastants. PhD dissertation. Wageningen University, Wageningen, Netherlands, 1988.
  18. Schifferstein HNJ. Sensory interactions in mixtures of tastants. PhD dissertation. Wageningen University, Wageningen, Netherlands, 1992.
  19. Maes F, Collignon A, Vandermeulen D, Marchal G, Suetens P. Multimodality image registration by maximization of mutual information. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 1997;16:187–98.
  20. Matsuda M, Liu Y, Mahankali S, et al. Altered hypothalamic function in response to glucose ingestion in obese humans. Diabetes 1999;48:1801–6.
  21. Cho ZH, Son YD, Kang CK, Han JY, Wong EK, Bai SJ. Pain dynamics observed by functional magnetic resonance imaging: differential regression analysis technique. J Magn Reson Imaging 2003;18:273–83.
  22. Grill HJ, Berridge KC, Ganster DJ. Oral glucose is the prime elicitor of preabsorptive insulin secretion. Am J Physiol 1984;246:R88–95.
  23. Sawchenko PE, Brown ER, Chan RK, et al. The paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus and the functional neuroanatomy of visceromotor responses to stress. Prog Brain Res 1996;107:201–22.
  24. Herman JP, Cullinan WE. Neurocircuitry of stress: central control of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical axis. Trends Neurosci 1997;20:78–84.
  25. Patterson SM, Matthews KA, Allen MT, Owens JF. Stress-induced hemoconcentration of blood cells and lipids in healthy women during acute psychological stress. Health Psychol 1995;14:319–24.
  26. Levin JM, Frederick BB, Ross MH, et al. Influence of baseline hematocrit and hemodilution on BOLD fMRI activation. Magn Reson Imaging 2001;19:1055–62.
  27. Rose RM, Hurst MW. Plasma cortisol and growth hormone responses to intravenous characterization. J Human Stress 1975;1:22–36.
  28. Meeran K, Hattersley A, Mould G, Bloom SR. Venepuncture causes rapid rise in plasma ACTH. Br J Clin Pract 1993;47:246–7.
Received for publication April 4, 2005. Accepted for publication July 18, 2005.


作者: Paul AM Smeets
医学百科App—中西医基础知识学习工具
  • 相关内容
  • 近期更新
  • 热文榜
  • 医学百科App—健康测试工具