点击显示 收起
the Department of Neurosurgery, Iwate Medical University (Y.O., K.O., A.O.), Morioka, Japan
Department of Clinical Psychology, Tochinai Daini Hospital (K.Y.), Morioka, Japan.
Abstract
Background and Purpose— This prospective study investigated whether surgery for unruptured intracranial aneurysms (UIAs) affects cognitive function and cerebral blood flow (CBF).
Methods— Cognitive tests using the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised, Wechsler Memory Scale, Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure test, and CBF measurements using single-photon emission computed tomography were performed before and after surgery for UIAs in 44 patients 70 years of age.
Results— Group-rate analysis showed the verbal intelligence quotient (IQ), performance IQ, full-scale IQ, and recall trial scores of the Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure test all increased significantly after surgery, whereas the Wechsler Memory Scale and copy trial scores of the Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure test were not significantly different. Event-rate analysis demonstrated that no patient showed impaired cognition. There was no significant difference between CBF before and after surgery.
Conclusions— Surgical repair for UIAs does not impair cognition or CBF in patients without postoperative restrictions in lifestyle.
Key Words: cognition intracranial aneurysm surgery
Introduction
The indications for the treatment of unruptured intracranial aneurysms (UIAs) continue to generate controversy. Most UIAs can be treated with acceptably low morbidity and mortality.1 The International Study of Unruptured Intracranial Aneurysms (ISUIA) suggested that the high surgical morbidity was largely attributable to impaired cognitive status.2 Cerebral blood flow (CBF) measurements have shown that reduction of CBF because of brain retraction is associated with potential cognitive dysfunction after surgical repair for UIAs.3 The present study measured CBF using single-photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) and assessed cognitive function using standard tests after surgical repair of UIAs.
Subjects and Methods
Patient Population
This prospective study was based on a series of patients treated for UIAs at our institution between April 1998 and August 2000. The inclusion criteria for this study were: UIA in the anterior circulation; surgical obliteration of the UIA; and age 70 years. The exclusion criteria were: no informed consent and obvious cognitive impairments (compression of brain by large UIA, previous premorbid psychiatric history, and brain injury caused by surgery for UIAs resulting in a modified Rankin Scale score of >1). Ethical approval was granted by the institution.
Surgical Procedures
A standard pterional approach using the operating microscope was performed after widely opening the Sylvian fissure. Self retractors were intermittently used on the frontal and temporal lobes. Cerebral veins were carefully dissected and preserved. All operations were performed or supervised by a senior author (A.O.).
Cognitive Tests and CBF Measurement
The cognitive test battery consisted of the Japanese translation of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R),4 the Japanese translation of the Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS),5 and the Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure test (ROCF).6 Cognitive tests were performed by a single trained neuropsychologist (K.Y.). Regional CBF was determined using 133Xe inhalation and SPECT. Quantitative CBF maps were reconstructed using the Kanno–Lassen method.7 An investigator unaware of the patient clinical data manually drew an irregular region of interest covering the entire cerebral hemisphere. The CBF was determined in the cerebral hemisphere ipsilateral to the surgical approach. The mean value of the bilateral hemispheres was used if the patient underwent bilateral pterional approaches.
Cognitive tests and CBF measurements were performed 1 month before and 1 month after surgery. If multiple aneurysms required multiple operations, the second examinations were performed after the last surgery.
Statistical Analysis
Neuropsychological performance was evaluated by group-rate analysis and by event-rate analysis. Group-rate analysis used the paired t test to analyze the neuropsychological scores. Event-rate analysis calculated the SD for each test from all preoperative scores, and a cognitive deficit was defined as a fall of 1 SD in the postoperative score compared with the preoperative score for individual patients.8 A patient with deficits in 2 test scores was considered to have postoperative cognitive impairment. The change in CBF before and after surgery was analyzed using the paired t test. Statistical significance was set at the P<0.05 level.
Results
Surgical complications occurred in 2 patients (parent artery occlusion and brain contusion), resulting in modified Rankin Scale scores of 2 and 5. Forty-four patients were entered in this study during the study period: 17 men and 27 women aged 32 to 70 years (mean age 56.8 years). The UIAs were found incidentally in 30 patients, and the remaining cases were found by other reasons (manifested as visual impairment in 2 patients and cerebral stroke in 12 patients). The mean interval between the onset of stroke and surgery for UIAs was 232±56 days.
Group-rate analysis of the neuropsychological tests is summarized in Table 1. The verbal intelligence quotient (IQ), performance IQ, full-scale IQ, and recall trial scores of the ROCF increased significantly after surgery. The WMS and copy trial scores of the ROCF showed no statistical difference before and after surgery. Event-rate analysis showed no patient developed postoperative cognitive impairment. There was no significant difference between CBF before and after surgery (Table 2).
Discussion
The present study demonstrated that surgical treatment of UIAs does not impair cognitive function or CBF in patients without postoperative restrictions in lifestyle.
Previous studies of cognition after surgery for UIAs have involved various study designs. The ISUIA lacks data of cognitive change before and after surgery.2 Another study defined impaired cognition as a decrease in postoperative test scores by 1 points, which may overestimate postoperative cognitive impairment compared with our study.3 Other previous cognitive study confirmed safe surgery for UIAs, although no CBF study was done.9 Our results confirmed that neither cognitive function nor CBF decreased after surgery. Previous research showed that surgical repair of UIAs does not affect CBF if the surgery can be completed with fine surgical manipulation.10 Thus, we believe that fine surgical manipulation preserves cognitive function.
Group-rate analysis found that the scores of the WAIS-R and ROCF improved after surgery. Improvement with practice can occur if patients are reassessed within 3 months.11 Other potential causes of the improved cognitive scores after surgery include a different cognitive test battery than those used in previous studies, the high preoperative level of anxiety affecting cognitive function,12 the inclusion of patients recovering from stroke, and the neuropsychologist being unaware of the clinical information. In conclusion, the present study demonstrated that surgical repair for UIAs does not impair cognitive function in patients without postoperative restrictions in lifestyle.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported in part by a grant-in-aid for advanced medical scientific research from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science, and Technology, Japan.
References
Raaymakers TWM, Rinkel GJ, Limburg M, Algra A. Mortality and morbidity of surgery for unruptured intracranial aneurysms: a meta-analysis. Stroke. 1998; 29: 1531–1538.
International Study of Unruptured Intracranial Aneurysms Investigators. Unruptured intracranial aneurysms: natural history, clinical outcome, and risks of surgical and endovascular treatment. Lancet. 2003; 362: 103–110.
Fukunaga A, Uchida K, Hashimoto J, Kawase T. Neuropsychological evaluation and cerebral blood flow study of 30 patients with unruptured cerebral aneurysms before and after surgery. Surg Neurol. 1999; 51: 132–139.
Shinagawa F, Kobayashi S, Fujita K, Maekawa H. Japanese Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised [in Japanese]. Tokyo, Japan: Nihon Bunka Kagakusha; 1990.
Koyama M. Clinical Psychology of Brain Damage [in Japanese]. Tokyo, Japan: Gakuensha; 1985: 48–54.
Spreen O, Strauss E. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1998: 341–363.
Kanno I, Lassen NA. Two methods for calculating regional cerebral blood flow from emission computed tomography of inert gas concentrations. J Comput Assist Tomogr. 1979; 3: 71–76.
Smith PL, Treasure T, Newman SP, Joseph P, Ell PJ, Schneidau A, Harrison MJ. Cerebral consequences of cardiopulmonary bypass. Lancet. 1986; 8485: 823–825.
Tuffiash E, Tamargo RJ, Hillis AE. Craniotomy for treatment of unruptured aneurysms is not associated with long-term cognitive dysfunction. Stroke. 2003; 34: 2195–2199.
Abumiya T, Sayama I, Asakura K, Hadeishi H, Mizuno M, Suzuki A, Yasui N, Shishido F, Uemura K. Regional effects of craniotomy on cerebral circulation and metabolism: PET study on the unruptured aneurysmal surgery [in Japanese with English abstract]. No Shinkei Geka. 1990; 18: 837–844.
Irvine CD, Gardner FV, Davies AH, Lamont PM. Cognitive testing in patients undergoing carotid endarterectomy. Eur J Vasc Endovasc Surg. 1998; 15: 195–204.
Hawkins KA, Sayward HK. Examiner judgement and actual stability of psychiatric inpatient intelligence quotients. Clin Neuropsychol. 1994; 8: 394–404.