Literature
Home医源资料库在线期刊中风学杂志2005年第36卷第10期

Effect of Posture on the Perception of Verticality in Neglect Patients

来源:中风学杂志
摘要:Perception。Perceptionofverticalityafterrecentcerebralhemisphericstroke。...

点击显示 收起

    the Service de Rééducation Neurologique and EA 2691, Hpital Swynghedauw (A.S., J.D., M.R.), CHRU de Lille
    Neurosciences Fonctionnelles et Pathologies (A.S., J.H., J.D.), FRE2726 CNRS, Université de Lille 2, CHRU de Lille
    URECA (A.S., Y.C.), Université de Lille 3, France.

    Abstract

    Background and Purpose— The anticlockwise (ACW) deviation of the visual and visuohaptic subjective verticals (SVs), known to occur in patients with right hemisphere lesion, is amplified by spatial neglect (N). These patients have only been assessed when sitting. We investigated the hypothesis that postural changes modulate visuohaptic SV deviation.

    Methods— Eight patients presenting with a right hemisphere lesion and spatial N were compared with 6 matched control subjects (C). In the dark, they had to rotate a luminous rod to put it at the vertical in 4 conditions: (1) sitting with plantar sole support; (2) sitting without plantar sole support; (3) sitting with legs extended on a support; and (4) supine position.

    Results— N patients showed a significant ACW deviation (–4.5°) of the SV compared with C subjects (+0.01°). The effect of body position depended on the group (P=0.022) because changes had definite effects in the N but not in the C group. In fact, the former showed a reduction of the ACW deviation, from the first to the fourth condition.

    Conclusions— Although the possible role of plantar and leg somaesthetic inputs remains to be thoroughly investigated, the modulation of gravitational inputs at trunk or vestibular level influences the SV deviation in N patients. This has to be put in relation with the modulation of N signs reported by other authors when passing from the sitting to the supine position.

    Key Words: hemispatial  neglect  posture  stroke  visual vertical

    Introduction

    The perception of body orientation relative to earth gravity requires integration of information provided by vestibular,1 visual,2 proprioceptive,3 and tactile signals.4 Continuous exposure to these covariant signals during static and dynamic postures provides multimodal configurations of indices corresponding to a great variety of body orientations.5

    The subjective vertical (SV) seems liable to interaction with body orientation. When assessed in the visual modality, SV corresponds to the orientation of a visual stimulus perceived as earth vertical (in the absence of relevant visual cues). This SV is known to depend on actual body orientation according to well-established effects. Although it is precise and accurate in upright healthy subjects, the visual SV is biased in roll-tilted subjects. In patients with right hemisphere injury, the visual SV was evaluated only when sitting, head upright, with methods providing6 or not providing 7,8 additional tactilo-kinesthetic cues. An anticlockwise (ACW) deviation occurred, which was amplified when the lesion causes a spatial neglect (N) syndrome.6,9

    In the present experiment, we investigated whether the modulation of gravitational inputs conveyed by the otolithic and somatosensory systems affects the deviation of the visual SV in N. In fact, we compared 4 body configurations, from sitting to supine conditions. Thus, the perception of SV of right brain–damaged patients with spatial N was assessed with a visuohaptic method when: (1) sitting with plantar sole support (SS); (2) sitting without plantar sole support (SNS); (3) sitting with legs extended (SLE); and (4) lying in supine position. In the second condition, the plantar input was suppressed; in the third, the proprioceptive input from legs was also suppressed; and in the last only, otolithic influence was changed.

    Subjects and Methods

    Subjects

    Patients were recruited in the neurological rehabilitation department of Le Centre Hospitalier Universitaire de Lille. They were informed of the protocol and signed an informed consent form before inclusion. All had experienced a relatively recent right hemispheric hemorrhagic or ischemic stroke, demonstrated by MRI or computed tomography scan.

    Patients with bilateral lesions, previous neurological or psychiatric disorders, impairment in primary visual perception, behavioral disorders, motor difficulties in the right upper limb, or psychotropic treatment and pusher syndrome were not included in the study. The pusher syndrome (ie, severe contralateral trunk deviation with active resistance to any attempt of external correction) was evaluated with the Scale for Contraversive Pushing10 and patients’ self-report of instability in a correct sitting position. They were regarded as pusher if they scored >3 on this scale and reported a feeling of rightward imbalance in vertical position, with and without perception of the visual environment.

    Spatial N was assessed using bell cancellation,11 line bisection,12 and scene copy13 tests. Patients were considered N when performance was pathological in 2 of the 3 tests.14

    Eight N patients (mean age 61.0 years) were compared with 6 right-handed healthy control subjects (C; 51.0 years). Hemianopia was observed in 5 of 8 cases and somatosensory disorders in 8 of 8 cases (Table). Two of these patients (D and F) participated in a previous study that compared SV in pitch and in roll after a right hemispheric stroke.6

    Clinical and Demographic Data of the Patients

    Experimental Setup

    The subject was installed on a chair or a medical device that enabled testing in sitting or supine positions. In fact, there were 4 conditions: (1) SS; (2) SNS; (3) SLE; and (4) supine position. The head, aligned with the trunk, was fixed by a headrest with lateral stabilizers; it was slightly raised when lying. The left arm was placed along the left thigh and maintained by a scarf in patients with severe hemiplegia. An abdominal strap helped to maintain a vertical position when sitting.

    A metal rod (25-cm long, 1.5-cm wide, 1.5-cm thick) that could rotate in roll plane (relative to the participant) was placed before a vertical black panel. A fixed metal disk (25 cm in diameter), centered on the rotation axis and inserted between the rod and the panel, avoided tactilo-kinesthetic cues that could be given by the panel. A precision potentiometer, mounted in the rotation axis, was connected to an electronic device that displayed the angle between the rod and the objective vertical (error) in tenths of degrees. Positive values corresponded to clockwise deviations, negative values to ACW deviations. Rotation was limited on each side by a stop situated at 110°. Ten red diodes (2-cm long, 0.5-cm wide) were inserted in the rod. All parts of the apparatus (rod included) were centered relative to the midsagittal plane of the participant. The distance between the participant and the middle of the rod was similar in the 4 different conditions.

    Procedure

    Participants were tested individually in a quiet, dark room. During the task, only the rod was visible, and the participant had to set it to the vertical, holding it in the middle with the right arm half-extended (visuohaptic method). Time was not limited, but task completion was fast, whatever the group. Three initial tilts of the rod (0, –45, and +45°) were used twice. Thus, there were 6 repetitions per body position. The participant had his/her eyes closed during the setting of the initial tilt. No information was given on performance. Trials were separated by 20 seconds. The order of initial tilt of the rod and the order of positioning were counterbalanced.

    Statistical Analyses

    For each participant and each body position, the 6 repetitions recorded were averaged. The ANOVA (Statistica software) performed on the resulting means included the factors group (2 groups) and body position (4 positions). Post hoc analyses used the Newman–Keuls test. The -risk was fixed at P<0.05.

    Results

    The effect of group was significant (F(1,12)=10.91; P<0.001). Healthy participants accurately adjusted the rod to the vertical (mean±SD: 0.01°±0.3°). N patients inclined the rod in the ACW direction (–4.5°±1.6°). The significant groupxbody positions interaction (F(3,36)=3.07; P=0.022) revealed that the posture effect differed between groups. As the Figure suggests, posture did not affect the SV of C group. On the contrary, the N group presented with a decrease in the ACW deviation, from the SS to the SNS, the SLE, and the supine positions; the linear trend was significant (F(1,7)=9.32; P=0.018) and explained 95% of the variance of body position factor. Post hoc analysis showed that the visuohaptic SV in the SS, SNS, and SLE positions was significantly different from that observed in the supine position (P<0.05).

    Visuohaptic SV as a function of the group and body position; reduction of the ACW deviation in patients with N from the sitting (SS) to the supine (S) position compared with C.

    Discussion

    The aim of the study was to assess the role of otolithic and somatosensory afferences in the perception of the visuohaptic SV among N patients. We showed that progressive change in body positioning, from the sitting to the supine position, associated with a reduction of graviceptive inputs, resulted in a progressive reduction of the visuohaptic SV bias of N patients.

    To the best of our knowledge, this effect has never been reported in the literature. In fact, the only available information is about the effect of body position on another clinical aspect of spatial N, ie, the ipsilateral deviation in line bisection. In 8 patients with N, Pizzamiglio et al15 investigated the influence of body position on the patients’ ability to bisect lines. This latter was measured when the body was either in an upright body position (sitting) or in a supine position. The rightward directional error found in the upright position (+6.3 cm or 5.1° of visual angle) was significantly reduced in the supine position (+2.9 cm or 2.4° of visual angle). A similar tendency, although not significant, was observed in a study by Karnath et al,16 with a reduced exploratory bias to the right when patients were pitched 30° backward. Data from Pizzamiglio et al15 suggest that this tendency could have been more pronounced if patients were placed in a full supine position.

    Pizzamiglio et al15 interpreted their finding as evidence that the gravitational input from the left and right otolithic system may be processed in a nonsymmetrical fashion by the neural circuits disrupted by a unilateral cerebral lesion. They assumed that the reduction of the otolithic input obtained when placing patients in the supine position reduced the ipsilesional distortion of a higher-order representation of space. In fact, the literature suggested that otolithic input is reduced in a supine position17 or when the head is pitched backward with subjects in a standing position.18

    The outcome of our study is consistent with the view that supine position results in a reduction of the otolithic influence. However, the somatosensory changes occurring when passing from the lying to the sitting position could also play a role. Indeed, an influence of somatosensory inputs was suggested by the effect of limb manipulation and deserves further investigation. The role of somatosensory afferences have been emphasized by other authors for explaining the modulation of the subjective visual vertical resulting from lateral body tilt in patients with bilateral peripheral vestibular lesion.19 By analogy with the hypothesis put forward about vestibular input,15 one may consider that reducing somatosensory input could reduce the deficit because of a processing system biased by the lesion.

    Concerning rehabilitation, the present study suggests that patients could benefit from performing exercises in supine position at the beginning of care, before they pass to the sitting position.

    References

    Bisdorff AR, Wolsley CJ, Anastasopoulos D, Bronstein AM, Gresty MA. The perception of body verticality (subjective postural vertical) in peripheral and central vestibular disorders. Brain. 1996; 119: 1523–1534.

    Howard IP, Childerson L. The contribution of motion, the visual frame, and visual polarity to sensations of body tilt. Perception. 1994; 23: 753–762.

    Kavounoudias A, Gilhodes JC, Roll R, Roll JP. From balance regulation to body orientation:two goals for muscle proprioceptive information processing Exp Brain Res. 1999; 124: 80–88.

    Anastasopoulos D, Bronstein A. A case of thalamic syndrome: somatosensory influences on visual orientation. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1999; 67: 390–394.

    Lackner JR. Human sensori-motor adaptation to the terrestrial force environment. In: Ingle DI, Jeannerod M, Lee DL, eds. Brain Mechanisms and Spatial Vision. Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff; 1985: 175–209.

    Saj A, Honoré J, Bernati T, Coello Y, Rousseaux M. Subjective visual vertical in pitch and roll after right hemisphere stroke. Stroke. 2005; 36: 588–591.

    Brandt T, Dieterich M, Daneck A. Vestibular cortex lesions affect the perception of verticality. Ann Neurol. 1994; 35: 403–412.

    Yelnik AP, Lebreton FO, Bonan IV, Colle FM, Meurin FA, Guichard JP, Vicaut E. Perception of verticality after recent cerebral hemispheric stroke. Stroke. 2002; 33: 2247–2253.

    Kerkhoff G. Multimodal spatial orientation deficits in left-sided visual neglect. Neuropsychologia. 1999; 37: 1387–1405.

    Karnath HO, Johannsen L, Broetz D, Ferber S, Dichgans J. Prognosis of contraversive pushing. J Neurol. 2002; 249: 1250–1253.

    Gauthier L, Dehaut F, Joanette Y. The Bells Test: a quantitative and qualitative test for visual neglect. Int J Clin Neuropsychol. 1989; 11: 49–54.

    Schenkenberg T, Bradford DC, Ajax ET. Line bisection and unilateral visual neglect in patients with neurological impairment. Neurology. 1980; 30: 509–517.

    Ogden JA. Anterior-posterior interhemispheric differences in the loci of lesions producing visual hemineglect. Brain Cogn. 1985; 4: 59–75.

    Rousseaux M, Beis JM, Pradat-Diehl P, Martin Y, Bartolomeo P, Bernati T, Chokron S, Leclercq M, Louis-Dreyfus A, Marchal F, Perennou D, Prairial C, Rode G, Samuel C, Sieroff E, Wiart L, Azouvi P. Présentation d’une batterie d’évaluation de la négligence spatiale. Normes et effets de l’ge, du niveau d’éducation et du sexe. Revue Neurologique (Paris). 2001; 157: 1385–1400.

    Pizzamiglio L, Vallar G, Doricchi F. Gravity and hemineglect. NeuroReport. 1995; 7: 370–371.

    Karnath HO, Fetter M, Niemeier M. Disentangling gravitational, environmental, and egocentric reference frames in spatial neglect. J Cognit Neurosci. 1998; 10: 680–690.

    Howard IP. Human Visual Orientation. New York, NY: Wiley; 1982.

    Diener HC, Dichgans J. On the role of vestibular, visual, and somatosensory information for dynamic postural control humans. Prog Brain Res. 1988; 76: 253–262.

    Bronstein AM, Yardley L, Moore AP, Cleeves L. Visually and posturally mediated tilt illusion in Parkinson’s disease and in labyrinthine defective subjects. Neurology. 1996; 47: 651–656.

作者: Arnaud Saj, PhD; Jacques Honoré, PhD; Jessy Davro 2007-5-14
医学百科App—中西医基础知识学习工具
  • 相关内容
  • 近期更新
  • 热文榜
  • 医学百科App—健康测试工具