Literature
Home医源资料库在线期刊中风学杂志2006年第37卷第2期

Ineffectiveness of Prism Adaptation on Spatial Neglect Signs

来源:中风学杂志
摘要:KeyWords:neglectrehabilitationstrokeIntroductionSpatialneglect(SN)isamainconsequenceofrighthemispherestroke。Prismadaptation(PA)toarightwardopticaldeviationcouldreduceSNmanifestationsandespeciallyimproveperformanceinarticleandpenciltests1,2anderrorsinreadi......

点击显示 收起

    the Service de Rééducation Neurologique and EA 2691, Hpital Swynghedauw, CHRU de Lille, France.

    Abstract

    Background and Purpose— To evaluate the effect of prism adaptation (PA) on spatial neglect signs.

    Methods— Ten patients (hemianopia in 6) and 8 control subjects were included. Tasks were reading single words, nonwords, and a text, bell cancellation, line bisection, and scene drawing, before and after adaptation, with either deviating or neutral prisms.

    Results— Errors were more frequent in patients but without any specific effect of PA. We found partial improvement at the late sessions, independent of prisms.

    Conclusions— We did not confirm the efficacy of PA on spatial neglect.

    Key Words: neglect  rehabilitation  stroke

    Introduction

    Spatial neglect (SN) is a main consequence of right hemisphere stroke. Prism adaptation (PA) to a rightward optical deviation could reduce SN manifestations and especially improve performance in article and pencil tests1,2 and errors in reading texts,1 single words,2,3 and nonwords.3 However, the efficacy of PA has been discussed recently.4,5

    We re-evaluated the efficacy of PA on reading and other clinical tests by comparing the effect of deviating and neutral prisms in the same patients.

    Subjects and Methods

    Subjects

    Ten right-handed SN patients were recruited consecutively after a recent (mean poststroke delay 54.3 days; range 17 to 102) right hemisphere stroke (middle cerebral artery infarct 8; subcortical hemorrhage 2). Each gave written informed consent to participate in the study. They were selected for their pathological performance6 in >1 of the following tests: bell cancellation, line bisection (20 cm long), and Ogden scene copy. All presented with spatial dyslexia in text reading (116 words).6 The severity of SN was most often severe (bell omissions >12 of 15 on the left side in 8 cases; right deviation >20 mm in 8 cases). Hemianopia was observed at clinical and instrumental examination (automatic perimetry) in 6 (neglect and hemianoptic: N+H+group) and absent in 4 (neglect and nonhemianoptic: N+H–group). They were 5 males and 5 females (mean age 55.5 years; education level 9.9 years). They were compared with 8 matched (age and education level) normal control subjects.

    Tests

    We first used conventional clinical SN test: bell cancellation (left–right omissions), line bisection (20 cm; right deviation in mm), and Ogden scene drawing (from 0: omission of the left tree, barrier, house, and left part of the right tree; to 10: perfect reproduction).

    A special emphasis was given to reading tests. Words (list A 48; list B 48) were concrete substantives, balanced according to 3 main factors: type (noncompound, prefixed, suffixed, compound words), length (5 to 6, 7 to 8, 9 to 10 letters), and frequency in the written language (high, low). Lists A and B were equivalent and presented alternatively. Nonwords (72) were constructed from the high-frequency words by single letter substitution or addition in the left or the right part of letter strings. Items were presented in a fixed semirandom order and written in lowercase "courier" fonts on an 8x15-cm card. We analyzed the percentage of errors. We also presented a text of 130 words (size font 20; "courier"; 10 lines of 27.3 cm; 9 loosely related sentences) written on a horizontal A4 sheet and evaluated the difference between left and right omissions (%).

    Procedure

    Each test was presented during 6 sessions of 35 minutes: pretest 1 (baseline), pretest 2 (1 hour pre-PA), post-test 1 (5 minutes after PA), post-test 2 (3 hours after PA), post-test 3 (1 day after PA), and post-test 4 (3 days after PA).

    PA was done successively (1-week interval, counterbalanced order) with prisms deviating visual perception at 10° toward the right side or with neutral prisms (Société Peter, Lyon). During PA, subjects had to point repeatedly with the right index finger to red or green dots placed at +10° or –10° from the body midline (60 cm). At least 50 trials were performed until target was reached without final hesitation.1 A mask prevented subjects from viewing their arm. Immediately after PA, they were tested with 2 to 3 trials to ascertain leftward deviation more than –2.9° (3 cm) when pointing with the eyes closed to a dot previously seen, facing their body midline. The mean deviation was –4.8° (range –3.8° to –10.4°).

    Statistical Analyses

    We used repeated-measure ANOVAs, with group as between-subject factor (N+H+, N+H–, controls) and prism and session as within-subject factors. Post hoc analyses used the Newman–Keuls test. The  risk was P0.05.

    Results

    In conventional SN tests, the group effect was always significant. Compared with controls, SN patients from both groups showed significant higher left–right omissions in the bell cancellation test (N+H+8.9 of 15; N+H–9.4; controls 0.02; P=0.0001), right deviation in line bisection (N+H+31 of 100 mm; N+H–17; controls 0.06; P=0.0007), and left omissions in the Ogden scene drawing (N+H+6.6 of 10; N+H–9.2; controls 10; P=0.006). But the main effects of prism and session and the interactions with group were never significant.

    In reading single words, factor group showed a clear tendency to significance (P=0.052) because errors (%) were more frequent in both patient groups than controls (N+H+ 40.0%; N+H–11.9%; controls 0.4%). Errors decreased with sessions (P=0.001), and the groupxsession interaction (P=0.002) was explained by the more severe reduction at post-tests 3 and 4 in both patient groups (Figure ). However, the groupxsessionxprism interaction was not significant, showing that this phenomenon was present with each prism. The main effect of prism was not significant.

    Evolution of percentage of errors in reading words in the 3 groups.

    In reading single nonwords, the group effect (P=0.009) was related to more frequent errors in both patient groups than controls (N+H+52.7%; N+H–28.2%; controls 1.8%). We found a main effect of session (P=0.009) because of a reduction of errors at post-tests 3 and 4. But the main effect of prism and the groupxsessionxprism interaction were not significant.

    For reading a text, the group effect was significant (P=0.0001) on left–right omissions, which were greater in patients than controls (global: N+H+54.0%; N+H–4.2%; controls 0.05%). The main effects of the other factors and the interactions were not significant.

    At an individual level, the negative results were relatively homogenous and independent of the severity of SN; no patient showed definite and selective improvement using deviating prisms.

    Discussion

    In this study of 10 stroke patients with SN, we failed to confirm the positive effect of PA, which has been reported on "paper and pencil" tests using symbol cancellation, line bisection, or scene drawing.1,2,7 Furthermore, PA did not improve neglect dyslexia. Reduction of errors in text reading has been reported in 1 study1 at the immediate and late (2 hours) post-tests. Positive effect has also been reported on single word and nonword reading.2,3 However, in a more recent report,4 authors did not find definite efficacy when patients had hemianopia in addition to SN.

    It seems unlikely that our negative results were related to prisms, PA training, or tests, which were similar to those used by other authors. The post-PA effect measured by index finger pointing was comparable.2–4 Furthermore, the absence of effect was not related to the presence of hemianopia.4 One important point was that we showed partial improvement with repetition of tests, independent of the quality of prisms, which was more evident at later sessions. It could be related to a learning effect or an increase of vigilance or sustained attention, both of which have an important relationship with SN.8 This could account for the improvement reported in previous series, in which the effect of deviating prisms was not systematically compared with that of neutral prisms in the same patients.

    It must also be emphasized that other authors did not find effectiveness on spatial attention tasks,5 and that poor generalization has been reported when PA used an upper-limb reaching task compared with a walking task.9 This later result suggests that PA be performed using more global tasks to draw clearer conclusions about its efficacy in SN patients.

    References

    Rossetti Y, Rode G, Pisella L, Farne A, Li L, Boisson D, Perenin MT. Prism adaptation to a rightward optical deviation rehabilitates left hemispatial neglect. Nature. 1998; 395: 166–169.

    Farné A, Rossetti Y, Toniolo S, Làdavas E. Ameliorating neglect with prism adaptation: visuo-manual and visuo-verbal measures. Neuropsychologia. 2002; 40: 718–729.

    Angeli V, Benassi MG, Ladavas E. Recovery of oculo-motor bias in neglect patients after prism adaptation. Neuropsychologia. 2004; 42: 1223–1234.

    Angeli V, Meneghello F, Mattioli F, Ladavas E. Mechanisms underlying visuo-spatial amelioration of neglect after prism adaptation. Cortex. 2004; 40: 155–156.

    Morris AP, Kritikos A, Berberovic N, Pisella L, Chambers CD, Mattingley JB. Prism adaptation and spatial attention: a study of visual search in normals and patients with unilateral neglect. Cortex. 2004; 40: 703–721.

    Rousseaux M, Beis JM, Pradat-Diehl P, Martin Y, Bartolomeo P, Bernati T, Chokron S, Leclercq M, Louis-Dreyfus A, Marchal F, Perennou D, Prairial C, Rode G, Samuel C, Sieroff E, Wiart L, Azouvi P. Batterie de dépistage de la négligence spatiale. Normes et effets de l’ age, du niveau d’éducation, du sexe, de la main et de la latéralité. Rev Neurol (Paris). 2001; 157: 1385–1400.

    Frassinetti F, Angeli V, Meneghello F, Avanzi S, Ladavas E. Long-lasting amelioration of visuospatial neglect by prism adaptation. Brain. 2002; 125: 608–623.

    Robertson IH, Tegner R, Tham K, Lo A, Nimmo-Smith I. Sustained attention training for unilateral neglect: theoretical and rehabilitation implications. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1995; 17: 416–430.

    Morton SM, Bastian AJ. Prism adaptation during walking generalizes to reaching and requires the cerebellum. J Neurophysiol. 2004; 92: 2497: 2509.

作者: Marc Rousseaux, MD, PhD; Thérèse Bernati, PhD; A 2007-5-14
医学百科App—中西医基础知识学习工具
  • 相关内容
  • 近期更新
  • 热文榜
  • 医学百科App—健康测试工具