Literature
首页医源资料库在线期刊美国临床营养学杂志2003年77卷第6期

Effect of zinc supplementation of pregnant women on the mental and psychomotor development of their children at 5 y of age

来源:《美国临床营养学杂志》
摘要:TsunenobuTamura,RobertLGoldenberg,SharonLRamey,KathleenGNelsonandVictoriaRChapman1FromtheDepartmentsofNutritionSciences(TT),ObstetricsandGynecology(RLGandVRC),andPediatrics(KGN)andtheCivitanInternationalResearchCenter(SLR),UniversityofAlabamaatBirmingham。......

点击显示 收起

Tsunenobu Tamura, Robert L Goldenberg, Sharon L Ramey, Kathleen G Nelson and Victoria R Chapman

1 From the Departments of Nutrition Sciences (TT), Obstetrics and Gynecology (RLG and VRC), and Pediatrics (KGN) and the Civitan International Research Center (SLR), University of Alabama at Birmingham.

2 Supported by grant HD32901 from the NIH and by research contract DHHS 282-92-0055 from the Agency for Health Care Policy.

3 Reprints not available. Address correspondence to T Tamura, Department of Nutrition Sciences, 218 Webb, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL 35294. E-mail: tamurat{at}uab.edu.


ABSTRACT  
Background: A negative effect of prenatal zinc deficiency on brain function has been well established in experimental animals, but this association in humans is controversial.

Objective: We evaluated the effect of prenatal zinc supplementation on the mental and psychomotor development of 355 children whose mothers participated in a double-blind trial of zinc supplementation that resulted in increased head circumference and birth weight.

Design: The children took 6 tests—the Differential Ability Scales, Visual Sequential Memory, Auditory Sequential Memory, Knox Cube, Gross Motor Scale, and Grooved Pegboard tests—at a mean age of 5.3 y. The scores were compared between the children of women who received a daily oral dose of 25 mg Zn during the second half of pregnancy and the children of women who received placebo.

Results: There were no differences in the test scores of neurologic development between the 2 groups. We analyzed the scores in 4 subgroups on the basis of maternal body mass index, because the increases in birth weight and head circumference due to the supplementation occurred only in the children of women with a body mass index (in kg/m2) < 26.0 in the original trial. No differences in the scores were found between these subgroups.

Conclusions: Zinc supplementation of women in the latter half of pregnancy had no effect on the neurologic development of their children at age 5 y. It is not known whether our findings of no positive effect in the population with apparently inadequate zinc nutriture can be readily extrapolated to other populations.

Key Words: Mental and psychomotor development • prenatal zinc supplementation • fetal zinc nutriture • head circumference • birth weight


INTRODUCTION  
Zinc is known to be essential for the normal growth and development of the fetus. A deficiency of this nutrient in pregnant animals has been shown to result in malformations and abnormal development and functioning of the central nervous system of the offspring (1–4). Many investigators have evaluated the association between fetal zinc nutriture and brain development in early life and established a negative effect of prenatal zinc deficiency on the brain function of experimental animals. Adverse consequences include reduced activity and responsiveness; impaired learning ability, attention, and memory; and increased aggressiveness. Several possible mechanisms for this association have been proposed (5, 6). During the past decade, many trials were performed to evaluate the effect of postnatal zinc supplementation on mental and psychomotor development during infancy and childhood; however, these investigations provided conflicting results (7–15). Studies on the relation between prenatal zinc nutriture and brain development in fetuses, infants, or children are extremely scarce (16–18).

We showed, in a double-blind clinical trial, that daily oral supplementation with 25 mg Zn during the second half of pregnancy resulted in a significantly increased birth weight in the infants of a population of African American women of low socioeconomic status (SES) who had low plasma zinc concentrations (19). We also observed a significantly greater head circumference in neonates born to women who received zinc supplementation than in neonates born to those who did not, and we thought that this finding could be reflective of increased brain size. On the basis of that finding, we hypothesized that the larger head size and possibly enhanced brain growth associated with prenatal zinc supplementation would lead to better mental and psychomotor development of children at a later age. We tested this hypothesis by evaluating the effect of prenatal zinc supplementation on the mental and psychomotor development of children at age 5 y. Furthermore, in the original study, the selection of women who had low plasma zinc concentrations at the first prenatal visit resulted in a high mean prepregnancy body mass index (BMI; in kg/m2) of 28.0. We evaluated the effect of zinc supplementation by dividing our population into 2 subgroups according to maternal BMI. The supplementation resulted in a significant increase in birth weight (248 g) and head circumference (0.7 cm) in newborns of women with a BMI < 26.0, whereas the effect was not significant in newborns of women with a BMI ≥ 26.0 (19). Therefore, we analyzed the effect of zinc supplementation on developmental test scores in these 4 subgroups.


SUBJECTS AND METHODS  
Subjects
The mothers of the children evaluated in this study were selected from 580 participants in a double-blind trial carried out between 1990 and 1993 to evaluate the effect of prenatal zinc supplementation on fetal growth in the Birmingham area (19). These women were originally selected because their plasma zinc concentrations at the first prenatal visit were below the established median for gestational age in a population from which > 3700 pregnant women were screened (20). After a computer-generated random number assignment, 294 pregnant women received a daily oral dose of 25 mg Zn as zinc sulfate (zinc group) and the remaining 286 received placebo (placebo group) starting at 19 wk of gestation. Zinc supplementation resulted in a significantly greater birth weight (126 g increase) and head circumference (0.4 cm increase) among the infants of the women in the zinc group than were seen among the infants of the women in the placebo group (19). All mothers were African Americans who received their prenatal care through the local public health system and were eligible for the Women, Infants, and Children supplemental program. Of this population, < 5% breastfed their infants. All mother-child pairs in which the mothers agreed to their participation in the present study were included in the evaluation. However, because of an inability to locate some families and constraints on resources, a total of 355 children (178 girls) participated in the evaluation of mental and psychomotor development at a mean age of 5.3 y. We aimed at detecting 5-point differences in various tests with a power of 80% (P < 0.05). The study was reviewed and approved annually by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Alabama at Birmingham.

Mental and psychomotor tests
Mother-child pairs took the tests at a mean time of 5.3 y after delivery. Mothers completed the Home Screening Questionnaire, the Wide Range Achievement Test, and the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test for evaluations of their cognitive functions (21–23). At the same time, children were given tests including the Differential Ability Scales [nonverbal, verbal, and general conceptual ability (intelligence quotient, IQ)], Visual Sequential Memory, Auditory Sequential Memory, Knox Cube, Gross Motor Scale, and Grooved Pegboard (dominant and nondominant hands) tests (24–28). Comprehensive reviews of these tests have been published (29–31). In short, the Differential Ability Scales is a standardized test of intelligence to assess cognitive abilities. The tests of visual or auditory sequential memory are designed to assess visual or auditory memory span. The Knox Cube test is an index of attention span and short-term memory. The Gross Motor Scale evaluates the development of gross motor function, and the Grooved Pegboard test assesses manipulative dexterity. In selecting these tests for the present investigation, we considered the following issues: 1) tests were standardized for the age group of our subjects and had adequate reliability; 2) validity for assessing the construct of interest was a requirement; 3) instruments were used that included a representative number of lower-SES, African American children or statistical weighting of norms to be equivalent to a representative US population; 4) the attention and time limitations were considered appropriate for 5-y-old children and their mothers or caretakers; 5) the tests had the ability to detect subtle deficits in learning, behavior, or coordination in addition to major disabilities; and 6) the tests provided multiple approaches to testing. Evaluators with extensive training in the methodology administered these tests, and they did not have knowledge of the zinc supplementation of the mothers or of the neonatal conditions, including birth weight, of the children.

Statistical analysis
Data are presented as means ± SDs when appropriate. The differences in the test scores between the zinc and placebo groups and among the 4 subgroups based on maternal BMI with a cutoff of 26.0 (19) were evaluated with the use of Student’s t test with Bonferroni’s correction where appropriate. Furthermore, the test scores were compared after adjustment for factors that could influence test results using linear regression analyses, which included the child’s birth weight, gestational age at birth, and sex and the mother’s age, BMI, smoking status, alcohol and illicit drugs use, and scores on the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test and the Home Screening Questionnaire. The interactions between zinc supplementation and maternal BMI were analyzed with the use of two-factor analysis of variance.


RESULTS  
The comparison between the zinc and placebo groups for selected characteristics of mothers and children (at birth and at follow-up 5.3 ± 0.3 y later) is shown in Table 1. There were no significant differences in various characteristics of mothers and children between the groups except that gestational age at birth, birth weight, and head circumference were significantly higher in the children in the zinc group than in those in the placebo group. This observation was consistent with the findings in the original population, and it indicates that the mother-child pairs selected for the present investigation reasonably represented the original 580 pairs (19). Other anthropometric measurements of children at follow-up did not differ significantly between the groups (data not shown). For the 2 groups combined, the mean maternal age was 28.7 ± 5.8 y, and mothers delivered singleton infants at a gestational age of 38.7 ± 2.7 wk with a mean birth weight of 3190 ± 635 g. The mean scores on the Home Screening Questionnaire (37.4 ± 6.9), Wide Range Achievement Test (43.8 ± 7.7), and Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (73.1 ± 11.9) were similar to those of inner-city populations with low SES (32).


View this table:
TABLE 1 . Selected characteristics of mothers and children in the zinc and placebo groups  
Comparison of the scores on various tests of mental and psychomotor development between the children of the zinc and placebo groups at follow-up is shown in Table 2. There were no significant differences between the 2 groups, and there was no trend suggestive of any effect of zinc supplementation.


View this table:
TABLE 2 . Child developmental test scores in the zinc and placebo groups1  
In the original trial, the increases in birth weight and head circumference associated with zinc supplementation were significant only in newborns of mothers with a BMI < 26.0 (19). Therefore, each group was divided into 2 subgroups according to maternal BMI for evaluation of whether the effects of zinc supplementation on the mental and psychomotor development of children differed with maternal BMI. The characteristics of mothers and children in these 4 subgroups are shown in Table 3. There were no significant differences in the characteristics of mothers in both BMI categories and their children (at birth and follow-up) between the zinc and placebo groups. When the characteristics were compared between the 2 BMI categories (zinc and placebo groups combined), mothers with higher BMI were significantly (P < 0.0001) older (29.8 ± 5.9 y compared with 27.3 ± 5.3 y) and were significantly (P < 0.005) more likely to be multiparous (63% compared with 48%) than were mothers with a lower BMI. The mean height at follow-up of the children whose mothers had a higher BMI was significantly (P = 0.002) greater than that of the children whose mothers had a lower BMI (112.7 ± 5.7 compared with 110.6 ± 6.8 cm).


View this table:
TABLE 3 . Selected characteristics of mothers and children in the zinc and placebo groups subdivided by maternal BMI1  
As is shown in Table 4, in both maternal BMI groups, test scores did not differ significantly among children whose mothers received zinc or placebo. Furthermore, after adjustment for other factors known to affect cognitive scores, including the infant’s sex, birth weight, and gestational age at birth, the mother’s score on the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, and the presence of child care, a linear regression analysis indicated that none of the differences persisted between the zinc and placebo groups regardless of maternal BMI. These findings indicate that there was essentially no effect of daily oral zinc supplementation during pregnancy on the mental and psychomotor development of children evaluated at follow-up. All of the variables in Tables 3 and 4 were analyzed with the use of the two-factor analysis of variance with interaction, and we found that the variables of interest including general conceptual ability (IQ) and the Knox Cube test score had significant interaction terms between zinc supplementation and BMI (P = 0.029 and 0.044, respectively).


View this table:
TABLE 4 . Mental and psychomotor test scores of mothers and children in the zinc and placebo groups subdivided by maternal BMI1  

DISCUSSION  
In a double-blind clinical trial to evaluate the effect on fetal growth of zinc supplementation during the second half of pregnancy, we found that supplementation resulted in a significant increase in head circumference and birth weight in the newborns (19). The positive response to the supplementation by the women, who were all African Americans with low SES living in the Birmingham area, led us to believe that in general they had suboptimal zinc nutriture (19, 20). We postulated that the increased head circumference of their newborns reflected better prenatal brain growth and would result in better mental and psychomotor development of their children later in childhood. To test this hypothesis, we evaluated the mental and psychomotor development of the children when they were 5 y old. Contrary to our expectation, however, prenatal zinc supplementation did not have a positive effect on the mental and psychomotor development of these children (Table 2), whose overall cognitive performance was, on average, 1 SD below national norms. Thus, the null hypothesis was not rejected.

We observed that the increases in birth weight and head circumference secondary to zinc supplementation were more pronounced in newborns of mothers with a BMI < 26.0 in the original trial (19). Therefore, we further analyzed the test scores on the basis of maternal BMI to evaluate whether the effects of the supplementation on the mental and psychomotor development of children were limited to children born to mothers with a BMI < 26.0 (Table 4). The overall findings were, however, similar to those found in the 2 subgroups combined, regardless of maternal BMI (Table 2). Therefore, it is likely that the increase in head circumference, 0.3 mm in the present population (Table 1), seen in the infants born to mothers who received zinc did not result in an advantage in mental and psychomotor development in later life.

It has been well established in experimental animals that prenatal and postnatal zinc deficiency leads to malformation and abnormal development and functioning of the brain (1–3, 5, 6). However, human studies to evaluate the effect of postnatal zinc supplementation on the mental and psychomotor development during infancy and childhood provided conflicting results among populations presumably having inadequate zinc nutriture (4, 7–15). Furthermore, to our knowledge, only 2 studies of the association between zinc nutriture during pregnancy and cognitive and motor development appear in the literature (16, 17). In 1994, Kirksey et al (16) reported the positive relation of the intake of estimated available dietary zinc during pregnancy to neonatal behavior and the motor development at 6 mo of age in 30 Egyptian mother-infant pairs. In 1998, Merialdi et al (17) evaluated the effect of 15 mg of zinc supplementation during pregnancy on the neurobehavioral development of the fetuses of 55 Peruvian women. They electronically monitored fetal heart rate and movement patterns at 36 wk of gestation and found that zinc supplementation led to fewer episodes of minimal fetal heart rate variability and to increases in fetal movement (17). The findings of these studies suggest a positive association between prenatal zinc nutriture and early brain development. However, it may be problematic to compare our findings and those of these earlier reports involving physiologic and early motor development (16, 17), because our findings in a population with inadequate zinc nutriture were focused on more global indicators of children’s competence in later life. In contrast to these earlier 2 studies, Hamadani et al (18) recently reported as part of a large-scale Bangladeshi study that maternal zinc supplementation (30 mg/d) during pregnancy had no positive effect on the mental and psychomotor development of 13-mo-old Bangladeshi infants. Their original study was performed to evaluate growth and morbidity in infants born to mothers with generally poor nutritional status. The findings by Hamadani et al (18) are consistent with ours; however, we evaluated the children at age 5 y and they did so at age 13 mo, and it is known that evaluation at 5 y correlates better with longer-term mental development of children than does that at 13 mo. It is not clear, however, whether there is a longer-term effect of inadequate prenatal zinc nutriture on mental and behavioral outcome of these subjects in adult life, and that question is worth investigating.

Low SES is known to influence the mental and cognitive development of children. In the present study, however, our comparison of developmental status scores between the zinc and placebo groups is not confounded by SES, because all participants had low SES. Furthermore, the overall assessment of the developmental outcomes did not change after regression analyses adjusting for maternal SES. One possible explanation for the failure of prenatal zinc supplementation to improve the test scores at age 5 y is that our population of children was so educationally neglected that any positive prenatal effect was overwhelmed. The overall mean IQ of 82 in these children may support this possibility, as do the low scores related to the Home Screening Questionnaire. Therefore, our findings may be limited and may preclude generalization to populations with higher SES.

In summary, we found no effect of prenatal zinc supplementation during the second half of pregnancy on the mental and psychomotor development of children aged 5 y. Our findings are not consistent with the well-established negative effects of prenatal zinc deficiency on the biochemistry and function of the brain of experimental animals (5, 6). It was disappointing for us not to identify a positive effect in our population of children whose mothers had apparently inadequate zinc nutriture, despite the earlier reported benefit of an increase in head circumference and birth weight in response to zinc supplementation (18). It may be that the long-term positive effect of prenatal zinc supplementation on neurologic development, if any, was obscured by extremely poor environmental influences on this group of children. Therefore, we are not certain whether our findings can be readily extrapolated to other populations.


ACKNOWLEDGMENTS  
TT designed the study and wrote the manuscript; RLG (principal investigator) designed the study and interpreted the data; SLR (principal investigator) and KGN were responsible for conducting the mental and psychomotor tests; and VRC was responsible for statistical analyses. None of the authors had any financial interest or conflict of interest with the study.


REFERENCES  

  1. Hurley LS. Tetratogenic aspects of manganese, zinc, and copper nutrition. Physiol Rev 1981;61:249–95.
  2. Prohaska JR. Functions of trace elements in brain metabolism. Physiol Rev 1987;67:858–901.
  3. Tamura T, Goldenberg RL. Zinc nutriture and pregnancy outcome. Nutr Res 1996;16:139–81.
  4. Bhatnagar S, Taneja S. Zinc and cognitive development. Br J Nutr 2001;85:S139–45.
  5. Sandstead HH. Zinc:essentiality for brain development and function. Nutr Rev 1985;43:129–37.
  6. Golub MS, Keen CL, Gershwin ME, Hendrickx AG. Developmental zinc deficiency and behavior. J Nutr 1995;125:2263S–71S.
  7. Gibson RS, Vanderkooy PDS, MacDonald AC, Goldman A, Ryan BA, Berry M. A growth-limiting, mild zinc-deficiency syndrome in some Southern Ontario boys with low height percentiles. Am J Clin Nutr 1989;49:1266–73.
  8. Friel JK, Andrews WL, Matthew JD, et al. Zinc supplementation in very-low-birth weight infants. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 1993;17:97–104.
  9. Cavan KR, Gibson RS, Grazioso CF, Isalgue AM, Ruz M, Solomons NW. Growth and body composition of periurban Guatemalan children in relation to zinc status: a longitudinal zinc intervention trial. Am J Clin Nutr 1993;57:344–52.
  10. Sazawal S, Bentley M, Black RE, Dhingra P, George S, Bhan MK. Effect of zinc supplementation on observed activity in low socioeconomic Indian preschool children. Pediatrics 1996;98:1132–7.
  11. Bentley ME, Caulfield LE, Ram M, et al. Zinc supplementation affects the activity patterns of rural Guatemalan infants. J Nutr 1997;127:1333–8.
  12. Sandstead HH, Penland JG, Alcock NW, et al. Effects of repletion with zinc and other micronutrients on neuropsycholigic performance and growth of Chinese children. Am J Clin Nutr 1998;68(suppl):470S–5S.
  13. Ashworth A, Morris SS, Lira PIC, Grantham-McGregor SM. Zinc supplementation, mental development and behaviour in low birth weight term infants in northeast Brazil. Eur J Clin Nutr 1998;52:223–7.
  14. Hamadani JD, Fuchs GJ, Osendarp SJM, Khatum F, Huda SN, Grantham-McGregor SM. Randomized controlled trial of the effect of zinc supplementation on the mental development of Bangladeshi infants. Am J Clin Nutr 2001;74:381–6.
  15. Castillo-Durán C, Perales CG, Hertrampf ED, Marin VB, Rivera FA, Icaza G. Effect of zinc supplementation on development and growth of Chilean infants. J Pediatr 2001;138:229–35.
  16. Kirksey A, Wachs TD, Yunis F, et al. Relation of maternal zinc nutriture to pregnancy outcome and infant development in an Egyptian village. Am J Clin Nutr 1994;60:782–92.
  17. Merialdi M, Caulfield LE, Zavaleta N, Figueroa A, DiPietro JA. Adding zinc to prenatal iron and folate tablets improves fetal neurobehavioral development. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1998;180:483–90.
  18. Hamadani JD, Fuchs GJ, Osendarp SJM, Huda SN, Grantham-McGregor SM. Zinc supplementation during pregnancy and effects on mental development and behaviour of infants: a follow-up study. Lancet 2002;360:290–4.
  19. Goldenberg RL, Tamura T, Neggers Y, et al. The effect of zinc supplementation on pregnancy outcome. JAMA 1995;274:463–8.
  20. Tamura T, Goldenberg RL, Johnston KE, DuBard M. Maternal plasma zinc concentrations and pregnancy outcome. Am J Clin Nutr 2000;71:109–13.
  21. Cooms CE, Gay EC, Fandal AW, Ker C, Frankenburg WK. The Home Screening Questionnaire, reference manual. Denver: Denver Developmental Materials, 1981.
  22. Jastak S, Wilkinson GS. Wide Range Achievement Test (Revised). Wilmington, DE: Jastek Associates, 1984.
  23. Dunn LM, Dunn LM. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (Revised). Pine Circle, MN: American Guidance Service, 1981.
  24. Elliott CD. Differential Ability Scales. Cleveland: Psychological Corporation, 1983.
  25. Kirk SA, McCarthy JJ, Kirk WD. Illinois Test of Pycholinguistic Abilities – Revised. Champaign, IL: University of Illinois Press, 1968.
  26. Stone M, Wright B. Knox’s Cube Test. Chicago: Stoelting Company, 1980.
  27. Folio MR, Fewell RF. Peabody Developmental Motor Scales and Activity Cards. Allen, TX: DLM Teaching Resources, 1983.
  28. Wilson BC, Lacoviello JM, Wilson JJ, Risucci D. Purdue Pegboard performance of normal children. J Clin Neuropsychol 1982;4:19–26.
  29. Keyser DJ, Sweetland RC, eds. Test critiques, vol 1. Kansas City: Test Corporation of America, 1984.
  30. Keyser DJ, Sweetland RC, eds. Test critiques, vol 5. Kansas City: Test Corporation of America, 1986.
  31. Keyser DJ, Sweetland RC, eds. Test critiques, vol 6. Kansas City: Test Corporation of America, 1987.
  32. Goldenberg RL, DuBard MB, Oliver SP, et al. Pregnancy outcome and intelligence at age five years. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1996;175:1511–5.
Received for publication May 16, 2002. Accepted for publication December 16, 2002.


作者: Tsunenobu Tamura
医学百科App—中西医基础知识学习工具
  • 相关内容
  • 近期更新
  • 热文榜
  • 医学百科App—健康测试工具